Fictional First Memories Psychology Questions
Akhtar, S., Justice, L.V., Morrison, C.M., & Conway, M.A. (2018).Fictional first memories.Psychological
Science, 29,1612 – 1619.doi: 10.1177/0956797618778831
Welcome to your assignment!Attached to this email you should find a .pdf copy of the article listed above.Your assignment is to read that article, and to answer the following questions to the best of your ability.This assignment is graded pass/fail; if you pass, you will receive one credit towards your PSY 101 research participation requirement.Also, if you are completing this assignment because you have been “locked” from signing up for other studies, passing will earn you an “unlock,” so that you can sign up for other studies.There is no penalty if you fail, but you do not get the research participation credit.Here are your instructions.
Read the attached article.
Write your answer to each of the listed questions, by hitting “Reply” in your email, and typing the answers into the body of the email.Your answers may be as long or as short as you need them to be.
Use complete sentences, and write in paragraph form.
Do not copy the work of anyone else, or share the work you do.Each of these assignments is stored electronically, and will be checked for plagiarism.
It may take up to 48 hours to get your assignment graded, depending on volume. Here are your questions! The term autobiographical memory refers to one’s long term memories of one’s own past.According to the article, what is the likely age range in which a developing human can form memories that will last to adulthood?Explain the evidence from Simcock and Hayne’s experiment is offered that supports this age range?Compare that estimate to the estimates from the subsequently reported two different studies of reports from young adults; at what ages do each of those studies suggest autobiographical memories occur?As part of your answer, be sure to compare “fragments” to “full” memories. The authors suggest that there study is different from prior studies in three ways.What are those differences? The Method section of a scientific paper is the place where the authors describe how they conducted their study, with enough detail that you could copy their procedure over again exactly, if you wanted to try to replicate their results.The last paragraph describes exactly which types of memories the participants were – and were not – allowed to use in the study.For this question, tell me the things participants were NOT allowed to do / include in the memory they reported.Why do you think the instructions excluded these sorts of situations? In trying to explain the odd results from their sample, the authors distinguish between fictional memories, false memories, and illusory memories.Explain the distinctions the authors make.
778831
research-article2018
PSSXXX10.1177/0956797618778831Akhtar et al.First Memories
ASSOCIATION FOR
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Research Article
Psychological Science
2018, Vol. 29(10) 1612–1619
© The Author(s) 2018
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0956797618778831
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618778831
www.psychologicalscience.org/PS
Fictional First Memories
Shazia Akhtar1, Lucy V. Justice2, Catriona M. Morrison3, and
Martin A. Conway1
1
Department of Psychology, City, University of London; 2Department of Psychology, Nottingham
Trent University; and 3Department of Psychology, University of Bradford
Abstract
In a large-scale survey, 6,641 respondents provided descriptions of their first memory and their age when they
encoded that memory, and they completed various memory judgments and ratings. In good agreement with many
other studies, where mean age at encoding of earliest memories is usually found to fall somewhere in the first half of
the 3rd year of life, the mean age at encoding here was 3.2 years. The established view is that the distribution around
mean age at encoding is truncated, with very few or no memories dating to the preverbal period, that is, below about
2 years of age. However, we found that 2,487 first memories (nearly 40% of the entire sample) dated to an age at
encoding of 2 years and younger, with 893 dating to 1 year and younger. We discuss how such improbable, fictional
first memories could have arisen and contrast them with more probable first memories, those with an age at encoding
of 3 years and older.
Keywords
first memories, age at encoding, age at retrieval, childhood amnesia, fictional memories, narrative memories, open
data
Received 9/18/16; Revision accepted 4/27/18
In many studies of the recall of earliest memories, the
first memory is found to date to the 3rd year of life,
typically about 3 years 4 months (Hayne, 2004; Kingo,
Berntsen, & Krøjgaard, 2013; Pillemer & White, 1989;
Rubin, 2000; Wang, Conway, & Hou, 2004). However,
also in many studies, there are always a few respondents who date their earliest memory to 2 years of age
and below (Hayne, 2004; Wells, Morrison, & Conway,
2014; see also Kingo, Berntsen, & Krøjgaard, 2013).
Indeed, there is some evidence that distinctive family
events, such as the birth of a sibling, might lead to the
formation and long-term retention of unusually early
first memories (Eacott & Crawley, 1998; Usher &
Neisser, 1993; but for a critique of the validity of such
“memories,” see Gross, Jack, Davis, & Hayne, 2013;
Loftus, 1993). Here, we had the unique opportunity
to sample a large group of adults across the life span
and to examine first memories in groups not usually
sampled, as previous studies typically have used only
young adults.
Interestingly, the study of memory development
similarly dates the emergence of first memories to the
age of about 3 to 4 years. Howe, Courage, and Edison
(2003), in their review of this research, concluded that
the processes underlying the ability to form autobiographical memories are functional by the 3rd year of
life, but they also note that other factors, including
sociolinguistic development, may further lengthen the
period during which full autobiographical memories
form (see also Bauer, 2007, 2015, and Howe, 2011a, for
recent reviews that reach similar conclusions). In one
of the only experimental studies, Simcock and Hayne
(2002) found that children exposed to an interesting
and novel event below the age of 3 years showed signs
of preverbal memory yet failed to translate the memory
into language both 6 months and 1 year later. Results
suggest that no enduring autobiographical memory of
the target event was formed in the first place, or
Corresponding Author:
Martin A. Conway, City, University of London, Department of
Psychology, Rhind Building, Northampton Square, London,
EC1V 0HB, United Kingdom
E-mail: Martin.Conway.1@city.ac.uk
First Memories
possibly, no memory that could be declaratively
reported was formed. The obvious implication is that
if children below the age of 2 to 3 years cannot form
full autobiographical memories, it is not possible for
adults to recall such memories from these ages.
Consistent with the findings from the study of the
development of memory are the outcomes from studies
of young adults recalling first memories. These variously date the emergence of first full autobiographical
memories to somewhere between the ages of 3 to 5
years. Rubin (2000), in a meta-analysis of over 11,000
early memories recalled by adults, found the emergence
of memories to date to about 3.4 years of age, with
virtually no memories falling below the age of 3. Moreover, of the 770 respondents who contributed memories
to this review, more than 76% (590) were younger than
30, meaning that the findings are limited to a comparatively young population (largely undergraduate university students). In contrast, Bruce, Dolan, and
Phillips-Grant (2000) found full first autobiographical
memories to date to 5 to 6 years of age and term
“memories” below this age “fragments” that were not
recollectively experienced when recalled. But even with
fragments, very few dated to below the age of 3 years.
The overwhelming evidence and theory is then that full
earliest autobiographical memories do not emerge
before about the age of about 24 to 36 months, and, if
anything, the onset of full autobiographical memories
may not be until later than this.
In the present study, we conducted the first largescale web-based survey of first memories (rather than
the more general category of early memories used in
many previous studies; see Rubin, 2000). Thus, the key
variable in the present study was respondents’ estimates
of their age when their first memory was formed: age
at encoding.1 Moreover, because this was a large-scale
study, we were able to sample across the full age range
and draw on the general population. Uniquely, the
survey was linked to a popular series of radio programs
on memory produced and broadcast by the British
Broadcasting Corporation (BBC) Radio 4 in the United
Kingdom (the programs can be listened to at http://
www.bbc.co.uk/radio4/memory/listenagain/). The survey is no longer live, but the questionnaire that was
used is included in the Supplemental Material available
online.
Method
In the first program of the radio series, the fourth author
introduced the idea that the program would conduct a
memory survey of various types of memories (earliest,
self-defining, and flashbulb memories) and report the
results of the survey in a later program. The audience
1613
was invited to log in to a memory website hosted by
the BBC that contained various sources of information
about memory and separate questionnaires for each of
the three types of memories to be sampled. The questionnaires always began with an information page outlining key instructions regarding the nature of the
to-be-sampled memory and an informed-consent box
to be checked, and minimal demographic data were
collected. Respondents were also informed that after
recalling their earliest memory, they would be asked to
answer some questions about the memory. For these
questions, they were instructed not to guess or infer
answers but to answer only if they actually remembered
the answer.
Respondents then moved to the next page of the
questionnaire proper. They were instructed to recall
and then type a title and description (in the box provided) of their very earliest memory. The title was to
be only a few words in length but of sufficient specificity that if they read it again, it would remind them of
the memory they had recalled. The memory description
was to be about a paragraph or so in length. The memory itself had to be one that they were certain they
remembered. It should not be based on, for example,
a family photograph, family story, or any source other
than direct experience. The memory had to be for a
specific one-off event that lasted no longer than minutes or hours. It was specifically emphasized that the
memory should not be of a routine or repeated event.
After entering the title and memory description, respondents were then asked to enter, in years, the age they
believed they were in the memory. Following this, the
respondents answered a series of questions regarding
the recollective qualities of the memory (see the Supplemental Material for details).
Results
There were 6,671 respondents who completed the survey. Inspection of the memory descriptions led to 166
responses being judged unusable because the memory
description was vague and lacked any specificity or
because it was explicitly stated that it was based on a
family story or photograph. Further, 39 memories
reportedly encoded over the age of 15 years were not
used because of their unusually late age at encoding,
and finally, respondents who gave their age group as
0 to 5 (n = 4) or 6 to 10 (n = 21) were removed because
of very low age (which were likely typographical
errors). Thus, a total of 6,441 memories were used, and
of these, 4,115 were from female respondents (63.9%;
mean age = 42.12 years, 95% confidence interval, CI =
[41.61, 42.6]) and 2,326 were from male respondents
(36.1%; mean age = 41.56 years, 95% CI = [40.89, 42.22]).
Akhtar et al.
1614
Figure 2 shows the frequency of age at encoding
across the sample. 2 What is immediately evident in
Figure 2 is that there were a large number of unexpectedly early memories, with 38.6% (2,487) of the sample
having what we term improbably early memories, dating to 2 years and younger (M = 1.64, 95% CI = [1.62,
1.66]); 52.3% (3,371) reporting what we term probable
memories, falling between age at encoding of 2 and 5
years (M = 3.65, 95% CI = [3.62, 3.67]); and the remaining 9.1% (583) reporting an age at encoding of 6 or
more years (M = 7.72, 95% CI = [7.55, 7.90]), which we
term improbably late memories.
Thus, the age at encoding of most memories fell in
the predicted range, 2 years to 5 years old; however,
the second largest group of memories had ages at
encoding that were unexpectedly early, falling in the
period of 2 years and less, and these were greater in
number than improbably late memories dating to 6
years and older. Despite this unexpected distribution,
the overall mean age at encoding of the whole sample
was 3.24 years (95% CI = [3.19, 3.29]), which compares
favorably with previous findings of the mean age of the
earliest memory that place it in the first half of the 3rd
year of life.
Next, we investigated whether age at encoding varied as a function of respondent age. In particular, we
wanted to determine whether the age at encoding
reported in most earliest-memory studies is somewhat
skewed as a result of the sampling of younger adults.
The sample was therefore split into two new groups: a
younger group comprising respondents within the 11–
15, 16–20, and 21–25 age groups (n = 1,228), similar to
the majority of participants sampled in Rubin’s (2000
10.0
Percentage of Respondents
7.5
5.0
2.5
11−15
16−20
21−25
26−30
31−35
36−40
41−45
46−50
51−55
56−60
61−65
66−70
71−75
76−80
81−85
86−90
91−95
96−100
0.0
Participant Age Group (years)
Fig. 1. Percentage of respondents across age groups.
Of the respondents, 82% (5,550) were UK nationals,
and the remaining 13.8% (891) resided in other parts
of the world. Figure 1 shows the distributions of memories across age groups of respondents and shows clearly
that memories were sampled across the life span.
Count of Memories
1,500
Improbably Early
Probable
Improbably Late
1,000
500
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Age at Encoding (years)
Fig. 2. Frequency of age at encoding grouped by memory type.
8
9
10+
First Memories
1615
Table 1. Percentage of Memories Within Each Semantic Category Across Memory Types
Memory type and category
Improbably early
Pram (baby carriage)
Family relationships
Feeling sad
Probable
Toy
Birth of a sibling
Home
School
Crying
Holidays
Dreams
Improbably late
Home
Activities
School
Percentage
of memories
Example
52
30
18
I was lying in my pram . . .
My parents were going on holiday and me and my elder sister . . .
I remember feeling very sad, my mum . . .
20
16
16
15
11
11
11
. . . my uncle had bought me a loopy loo doll. It was almost as . . .
. . . the arrival of my baby brother. When he was born and my . . .
. . . the front door opened directly into the kitchen which had . . .
. . . my first day at primary school, there was another little girl . . .
. . . I remember crying hysterically . . . I would not be comforted . . .
. . . we travelled to a holiday camp in Sussex on the Small Hythe . . .
. . . being potty trained in my dream . . .
59
26
15
In the winter of 1940 we lived in south London . . .
. . . playing football with my friends . . .
I attended the local school. The school remained open . . .
study), and an older group comprised of all remaining
respondents (n = 5,213). The mean age at encoding
was 3.56 (95% CI = [3.44, 3.68]) for the younger group
and 3.16 (95% CI = [3.11, 3.22]) for the older group.
These means were reliably different, t(1695) = 6.02,
p < .001, d = 0.19, 95% CI = [0.13, 0.25]), showing that
the older group had reliably earlier first memories than
the younger group. The mean age of the younger
group’s earliest memories was then more consistent
with previous studies using young adults, although we
note that in the present study, even some of this group
had memories dating to 2 years and below. 3
Memory Content
It is hypothesized that early memories are fragments of
memories (Bruce et al., 2000), lacking rich and detailed
descriptions. This was tested in the present study by,
first, assessing the word count of the memory descriptions as a function of memory group. A Poisson regression with planned comparisons (early vs. probable and
early vs. late) found no reliable difference in word
count between improbably early memories (M = 69.20,
95% CI = [67.02, 71.38]) and probable memories (M =
68.82, 95% CI = [66.87, 70.76]; p = .14, b = 0.007, 95%
CI = [−0.002, 0.017]), but improbably early memories
had a reliably shorter word count than improbably late
memories (M = 70.33, 95% CI = [65.78, 74.88]; p < .001,
b = 0.025, 95% CI = [0.011, 0.039]). Although reliably
different, memories across all three categories had negligible differences in word count (±1 word); thus, contrary to the suggestion that early memories are
fragments, the present findings show that they are similar in length to both probable and improbably late first
memories.
Second, the corpus of memory descriptions was further analyzed using the Alceste software (IMAGE, 2018)
for statistical analysis of textual data. This software
bridges quantitative and qualitative methods, analyzing
natural language using multivariate statistical methods
to identify groups of words, that is, phrases and sentences, that reliably cluster together across different
contexts. The resulting output provides categories of
dominant themes in the corpus that are required to be
named by the analyst. Separate analyses were performed on the descriptions of improbably early, probable, and improbably late memories, yielding a linguistic
profile for each memory group (Table 1).
In Table 1, it can be seen that 100% of descriptions
of improbably early memories fit into one of three
categories, the dominant category being memory
descriptions in which a pram (baby carriage) featured
across various contexts. We also note that the category
“birth of a sibling,” which has previously been identified as an event likely to give rise to very early first
memories (Eacott & Crawley, 1998; Usher & Neisser,
1993), did not feature in any of the improbably early
memories analyzed in the study corpus. In contrast,
100% of descriptions of probable memories were
accounted for by seven categories, all of which clustered around words and phrases referring to aspects of
childhood, and many descriptions featured toys in a
wide variety of contexts (see Table 1). Finally, 100% of
descriptions of improbably late memories decomposed
Akhtar et al.
1616
into three categories, with the dominant category featuring descriptions that mentioned home in a wide
variety of contexts. In summary, the linguistic analysis
of the memory descriptions found them to be age
appropriate; descriptions of improbably early memories
referred to events and activities from infancy, such as
being pushed in a pram or baby carriage; probable
memories referred to events and activities from early
childhood, for example, playing with toys; and improbably late memories often referred to events in the home,
such as family gatherings of various sorts (examples of
memories for each category are included in the Supplemental Material).
Discussion
The present findings pose a major conundrum: The
child and young adult research, as reviewed earlier,
concludes that earliest memories cannot exist below
about the age of 2 years and that there would be comparatively few memories below the age of 3 years. Yet
the main finding of the present survey of earliest memories, the largest such survey ever conducted, is that
2,487 (38.6% of the entire sample) of the earliest memories dated to when respondents were 2 years of age or
younger, with, astonishingly, 893 (13.9%) dating to 1
year or younger. These are what we have termed
improbable first memories and raise a question: How
best do we explain them? Below, we evaluate three
possible explanations: misdating, nature of the respondents, and the narrative and fictional nature of the “life
story” (Habermas & Bluck, 2000; Köber, Schmiedek, &
Habermas, 2015).
The misdating explanation
Dating of all autobiographical memories, including
childhood memories, is predominantly inferential, and
specific calendar and age dates are rarely retained in
long-term memory (Thompson, Skowronski, & Larsen,
1979). Thus, it is possible that some of the dates given
for first memories in the present study are incorrect
estimates; indeed, it would be remarkable were they
not. We assume, however, that such misdating is random rather than systematic and therefore represents
noise in the age-at-encoding measure. Nonetheless, a
plausible misdating account of the present findings
might propose that, for unknown reasons, almost 40%
of the sample systematically backward-telescoped their
estimates of the age at encoding of their first memories
(see Wang & Peterson, 2014, for evidence of forward
telescoping in estimates of earliest memories).
If the misdating account were correct, then it would
be expected that the improbable early memories would
be about events similar to those that were dated to 3
years and older. But this was not the case, and our
content analysis found that improbable first memories
were of events that related to infancy, whereas memories dating to 3 years and older (probable first memories) were of events related to childhood (see Table 1).
These findings of differences in the content of improbably early and probable first memories effectively rule
out the systematic misdating explanation.
The respondents: self-selection
The present sample of respondents differed from most
previous studies in that they consisted of individuals
from across the life span. Given that they freely
responded to the request to complete a web-based
memory survey, they were self-selected. Self-selection
is common in most psychological research; after all,
even students participating for course credit are selfselected. Random selection is typically not practically
possible, particularly given resource constraints. Nevertheless, a very large sample, even if self-selected, has
the advantage of very high power. In the present study,
power approached 1 for all effect sizes, far higher than
that in most psychology research and indeed in most
social science research.
Yet the possibility remains that there is some unique
aspect of this sample. One possibility is that members
of this group have thought about (i.e., rehearsed) their
past more than other groups, and in the course of so
doing have, perhaps implicitly or nonconsciously, generated cues that allowed them to access far earlier
memories than those accessed in previous studies. The
present findings suggest that this may occur more frequently in older than in younger adults. A problem for
this explanation, however, is that there were no differences in rated rehearsal between the older and younger
groups, both of whom indicated equal moderate levels
of rehearsal (see the Supplemental Material). Instead,
it may be that middle-aged adults have a more developed life story than younger adults—one that incorporates and constructs knowledge from, or about, infancy
(their own, possibly other people’s, possibly infancy in
general) into the form of memories or what we here
term fictional memories.
The life story and fictional memories
If the improbably early memories, memories that
research tells us cannot be formed at such young ages,
are largely of imagined rather than experienced events,
how do these fictional memories arise? Note that we
use the term fictional memories here rather than false
memories or illusory memories for a number of reasons.
First Memories
One is that the term false memories has a pejorative
aspect to it—false memories are negative, and the term
illusory memories suggests some sort of memory error.
We note that more recent work has found positive
aspects to false memories (see Howe, 2011b; Howe,
Wilkinson, Garner, & Ball, 2016; Schacter, Guerin, &
St. Jacques, 2011). Moreover, there may be adaptive
consequences of fictional memories more generally. For
example, in adulthood, preserving a positive and consistent self-narrative helps a person maintain a positive
self-image that can foster positive social interactions
with others, ones that arguably enhance the rememberer’s quality of life (see Ross & Wilson, 2000, 2003).
Fictional memories are then part of the life story and
may play a central, and positive, role in defining periods
of life or lifetime periods (Conway, 2005; Conway &
Pleydell-Pearce, 2000). It is particularly noteworthy that
all the memories we sampled (improbably early, probable, improbably late) included age-appropriate events,
and viewed overall, they give a picture of a life story
with successive early periods each with a distinctive
content.
Thus, in our analysis of the content of the descriptions of memories from the different ages at encoding
(see Table 1), we found that accounts dating to 2 years
and earlier contained details relating to infancy. Under
the three broad categories of pram, family relationships,
and feeling sad, these were details such as “an image
of my pram,” “being in my cot,” “in my push chair,”
“having my nappy changed,” and, even more implausibly, “the first time I walked,” “wanting to tell my
mother something before I could talk,” “the first word
I spoke,” and so on. On the basis of these descriptions,
we suggest that what people often have in mind when
“recalling” these improbably early memories is an
image (often visual) of an object or action possibly
dating to very early childhood. This might be based on
experience or derived from a photograph or a description (the rememberer may not be aware of the source
of the image or images). Other sources of details for
improbably early memories may derive from family
stories or history, for example, “the first word you spoke
was ‘X,’” “all you ever wanted to do when you were
little was walk,” and so on. These facts of infancy, possibly along with some visual fragments, form the basis
of remembering infancy: Their source is believed to be
or even experienced as being from these very early
ages and, accordingly, dated to those times. Thus, we
suggest that what a rememberer has in mind when
recalling fictional improbably early memories is an
episodic-memory-like mental representation consisting
of remembered fragments of early experience and some
facts or knowledge about his or her own infancy or
1617
childhood. In addition, further details may be nonconsciously inferred or added, such as that one was wearing a nappy (diaper) when standing in the cot. Such
episodic-memory-like mental representations come,
over time, to be recollectively experienced (Gardiner
& Richardson-Klavehn, 2000) when they come to mind,
and so for the individual, they quite simply are “memories,” memories that their content indicates date to a
particular time: infancy.
We suggest that improbably early first memories fall
in a larger class of fictional memories. Indeed, in the
constructive view of memory, all memories contain
some degree of fiction. For example, all memories are
time-compressed and therefore do not literally represent
the experience from which they derive. Similarly, all
memories contain details that are both consciously and
nonconsciously inferred. For example, Wells et al. (2014)
found that clothes in childhood memories were poorly
recalled. Nonetheless, respondents in that study recalled
that they had been clothed, and the same applies to
many other types of details, such as weather, time of
day, conversations, and so on, that are also (nonconsciously) inferred rather than remembered. Memories,
then, are part of a narrative of a person’s life, and the
way in which they correspond to experience and cohere
with other memories is complex and dynamic.
Note that we use the term narrative as it used by
Goldie (2012) in his account of narrative thinking,
which is an internal mental representation rather than
a publicly presented account. In this conception, the
personal value and significance of a fictional memory
resides in how coherent it is with other parts of autobiographical memory rather than with how well it corresponds to a previously experienced reality (see
Conway, 2005, and Conway, Loveday, & Cole, 2016, for
discussion of coherence and correspondence in autobiographical memory). Perhaps what is important when
it comes to questions of accuracy of a memory, from
any age, is the extent of fictionalization of details. In
the present study, the data indicate that very early fictional memories are more common in middle-aged and
older adults, and about 4 in 10 of this group have fictional memories for infancy. To a lesser degree, they
are also present in some younger people. Perhaps, the
life narrative or story, mainly for the middle-aged, needs
to extend (for reasons that are not yet understood but
possibly have to do with coherence and completeness
of the life narrative) to the very earliest years of life
and hence the emergence of improbably early fictional
first memories.
Action Editor
D. Stephen Lindsay served as action editor for this article.
Akhtar et al.
1618
Author Contributions
References
S. Akhtar worked as M. A. Conway’s postdoctoral research
assistant during this research and preparation of the manuscript. Although both made different contributions, both agree
that their contributions were equal. L. V. Justice assisted with
the statistical analyses, and C. M. Morrison assisted with the
initial preparation of the data. All the authors approved the
final manuscript for submission.
Bauer, P. J. (2007). Remembering the times of our lives:
Memory in infancy and beyond. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bauer, P. J. (2015). A complementary processes account of
the development of childhood amnesia and a personal
past. Psychological Review, 122, 204–231.
Bruce, D., Dolan, A., & Phillips-Grant, K. (2000). On the
transition from childhood amnesia to the recall of personal memories. Psychological Science, 11, 360–364.
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00271
Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of
Memory and Language, 53, 594–628.
Conway, M. A., Loveday, C., & Cole, S. N. (2016). The
remembering-imagining system. Memory Studies, 9, 256–
265.
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self memory
system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.
Eacott, M. J., & Crawley, R. A. (1998). The offset of childhood amnesia: Memory for events that occurred before
age 3. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127,
22–33.
Gardiner, J. M., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2000). Remembering
and knowing. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The
Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 229–244). Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Goldie, P. (2012). The mess inside: Narrative, emotion, & the
mind. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Gross, J., Jack, F., Davis, N., & Hayne, H. (2013). Do children
recall the birth of a younger sibling? Implications for the
study of childhood amnesia. Memory, 21, 336–346.
Habermas, T., & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The emergence of the life story in adolescence. Psychological
Bulletin, 126, 748–769.
Hayne, H. (2004). Infant memory development: Implications
for childhood amnesia. Developmental Review, 24, 33–73.
Howe, M. L. (2011a). The adaptive nature of memory and
its illusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
20, 312–315.
Howe, M. L. (2011b). The nature of early memory: An adaptive
theory of the genesis and development of memory. Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Edison, S. C. (2003). When
autobiographical memory begins. Developmental Review,
23, 471–494.
Howe, M. L., Wilkinson, S., Garner, S. R., & Ball, L. J. (2016).
On the adaptive function of children’s and adults’ false
memories. Memory, 24, 1062–1077.
IMAGE. (2018). Alceste [Computer software]. Retrieved from
http://www.image-zafar.com/Logicieluk.html
Kingo, O. S., Berntsen, D., & Krøjgaard, P. (2013). Adults’
earliest memories as a function of age, gender, and education in a large stratified sample. Psychology and Aging,
28, 646–653.
Köber, C., Schmiedek, F., & Habermas, T. (2015). Charac
terizing lifespan development of three aspects of coherence
in life narratives: A cohort-sequential study. Developmental
Psychology, 51, 260–275.
Acknowledgments
We thank the British Broadcasting Corporation (BBC) for
supporting this work and Katryn Hohl and Mark L. Howe
for comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. We
thank John Partington of the BBC, who created the original
website.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of interest
with respect to the authorship or the publication of this
article.
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information can be found at http://
journals.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.1177/09567976
18778831
Open Practices
All data have been made publicly available via Figshare and
can be accessed at https://figshare.com/articles/Fictional_
First_Memories/6115676. Materials for this study have not
been made publicly available, and the design and analysis
plans were not preregistered. The complete Open Practices
Disclosure for this article can be found at http://journals
.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.1177/0956797618778831. This
article has received the badges for Open Data. More information about the Open Practices badges can be found at http://
www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/badges.
Notes
1. Other rating measures of vividness, emotional intensity, and
memory perspective were also collected, but they were secondary measures and not found to be systematically related
to age at encoding. Consequently, they are reported in the
Supplemental Material available online.
2. The full data set can be accessed at https://figshare.com/
articles/Fictional_First_Memories/6115676.
3. As far as judgments of recollective qualities were concerned,
all memories, regardless of group, were of moderate vividness
and were rated as being recalled moderately often. Interestingly,
improbably early memories were more strongly associated with
an observer than a field perspective. See the Supplemental
Material for full analyses.
First Memories
Loftus, E. L. (1993). Desperately seeking memories of the first
few years of childhood: The reality of early memories.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 274–277.
Pillemer, D. B., & White, S. H. (1989). Childhood events
recalled by children and adults. In H. W. Reese (Ed.),
Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 21, pp.
297–340). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2000). Constructing and appraising
past selves. In D. L. Schacter & E. Scarry (Eds.), Memory,
brain, and belief (pp. 231–259). Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2003). Autobiographical memory
and conceptions of the self: Getting better all the time.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 66–69.
Rubin, D. C. (2000). The distribution of early childhood memories. Memory, 8, 265–269.
Schacter, D. L., Guerin, S. A., & St. Jacques, P. L. (2011).
Memory distortion: An adaptive perspective. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 15, 467–474.
1619
Simcock, G., & Hayne, H. (2002). Breaking the barrier?
Children fail to translate their preverbal memories into
language. Psychological Science, 13, 225–231.
Thompson, C. P., Skowronski, J. J., & Larsen, S. F. (1979).
Autobiographical memory: Remembering what and
remembering when. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Usher, J. A., & Neisser, U. (1993). Childhood amnesia and
the beginnings of memory for early life events. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 155–165.
Wang, Q., Conway, M. A., & Hou, Y. (2004). Infantile amnesia: A
cross-cultural investigation. Cognitive Sciences, 1, 123–135.
Wang, Q., & Peterson, C. (2014). Your earliest memory may
be earlier than you think: Prospective studies of children’s
dating of earliest childhood memories. Developmental
Psychology, 50, 1680–1686.
Wells, C., Morrison, C. M., & Conway, M. A. (2014). Adult
recollections of childhood memories: What details can
be recalled? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy
chology, 67, 1249–1261.