National University of Singapore R studio Question

One R studio Question.

Data Visualization
Data Visualization
A PRACTICAL INTRODUCTION
Kieran Healy
princeton university press
princeton and oxford
© 2019 Princeton University Press
Published by Princeton University Press
41 William Street, Princeton, New Jersey 08540
6 Oxford Street, Woodstock, Oxfordshire OX20 1TR
press.princeton.edu
All Rights Reserved
Library of Congress Control Number: 2018935810
ISBN 978-0-691-18161-5
ISBN (pbk.) 978-0-691-18162-2
British Library Cataloging-in-Publication Data is available
This book has been composed with open-source tools in Minion Pro,
Myriad Pro, and Iosevka Type.
Printed on acid-free paper. ∞
Printed in the United States of America
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
For the Llamanteriat, who saw it first.
Contents
Preface
xi
What You Will Learn
xii
The Right Frame of Mind
How to Use This Book
Conventions
2
xv
xvi
Before You Begin
1
xiv
xvii
Look at Data 1
1.1
Why Look at Data? 2
1.2
What Makes Bad Figures Bad?
1.3
Perception and Data Visualization
1.4
Visual Tasks and Decoding Graphs 23
1.5
Channels for Representing Data 26
1.6
Problems of Honesty and Good Judgment
1.7
Think Clearly about Graphs 29
1.8
Where to Go Next 31
5
14
Get Started 32
2.1
Work in Plain Text, Using RMarkdown
2.2
Use R with RStudio
2.3
Things to Know about R
2.4
Be Patient with R, and with Yourself
2.5
Get Data into R
49
35
38
48
32
27
viii

Contents
3
4
5
2.6
Make Your First Figure
2.7
Where to Go Next
51
52
Make a Plot 54
3.1
How Ggplot Works 54
3.2
Tidy Data 56
3.3
Mappings Link Data to Things You See
3.4
Build Your Plots Layer by Layer
3.5
Mapping Aesthetics vs Setting Them 63
3.6
Aesthetics Can Be Mapped per Geom 66
3.7
Save Your Work 68
3.8
Where to Go Next
56
59
71
Show the Right Numbers 73
4.1
Colorless Green Data Sleeps Furiously 74
4.2
Grouped Data and the “Group” Aesthetic
4.3
Facet to Make Small Multiples
4.4
Geoms Can Transform Data 80
4.5
Frequency Plots the Slightly Awkward Way 82
4.6
Histograms and Density Plots 85
4.7
Avoid Transformations When Necessary 88
4.8
Where to Go Next
74
76
91
Graph Tables, Add Labels, Make Notes 93
5.1
Use Pipes to Summarize Data 94
5.2
Continuous Variables by Group or Category
5.3
Plot Text Directly 115
5.4
Label Outliers
5.5
Write and Draw in the Plot Area 124
121
102
Contents
6
7
8
5.6
Understanding Scales, Guides, and Themes
5.7
Where to Go Next 131
125
Work with Models 134
6.1
Show Several Fits at Once, with a Legend
6.2
Look Inside Model Objects 137
6.3
Get Model-Based Graphics Right 141
6.4
Generate Predictions to Graph
6.5
Tidy Model Objects with Broom 146
6.6
Grouped Analysis and List Columns
6.7
Plot Marginal Effects 157
6.8
Plots from Complex Surveys
6.9
Where to Go Next 168
135
143
151
161
Draw Maps 173
7.1
Map U.S. State-Level Data 175
7.2
America’s Ur-choropleths 182
7.3
Statebins 189
7.4
Small-Multiple Maps 191
7.5
Is Your Data Really Spatial? 194
7.6
Where to Go Next 198
Refine Your Plots 199
8.1
Use Color to Your Advantage
8.2
Layer Color and Text Together
8.3
Change the Appearance of Plots with Themes 208
8.4
Use Theme Elements in a Substantive Way
8.5
Case Studies
8.6
Where to Go Next 230
215
201
205
211

ix
x

Contents
Acknowledgments
Appendix
233
235
1 A Little More about R
235
2 Common Problems Reading in Data 245
3 Managing Projects and Files
253
4 Some Features of This Book
257
References
Index
267
261
Preface
You should look at your data. Graphs and charts let you explore
and learn about the structure of the information you collect. Good
data visualizations also make it easier to communicate your ideas
and findings to other people. Beyond that, producing effective
plots from your own data is the best way to develop a good eye
for reading and understanding graphs—good and bad—made by
others, whether presented in research articles, business slide decks,
public policy advocacy, or media reports. This book teaches you
how to do it.
My main goal is to introduce you to both the ideas and the
methods of data visualization in a sensible, comprehensible, reproducible way. Some classic works on visualizing data, such as The
Visual Display of Quantitative Information (Tufte 1983), present
numerous examples of good and bad work together with some
general taste-based rules of thumb for constructing and assessing graphs. In what has now become a large and thriving field of
research, more recent work provides excellent discussions of the
cognitive underpinnings of successful and unsuccessful graphics,
again providing many compelling and illuminating examples
(Ware 2008). Other books provide good advice about how to graph
data under different circumstances (Cairo 2013; Few 2009; Munzer 2014) but choose not to teach the reader about the tools used
to produce the graphics they show. This may be because the software used is some (proprietary, costly) point-and-click application
that requires a fully visual introduction of its own, such as Tableau,
Microsoft Excel, or SPSS. Or perhaps the necessary software is
freely available, but showing how to use it is not what the book
is about (Cleveland 1994). Conversely, there are excellent cookbooks that provide code “recipes” for many kinds of plot (Chang
2013). But for that reason they do not take the time to introduce the
beginner to the principles behind the output they produce. Finally,
we also have thorough introductions to particular software tools
xii

Preface
and libraries, including the ones we will use in this book (Wickham
2016). These can sometimes be hard for beginners to digest, as they
may presuppose a background that the reader does not have.
Each of the books I have just cited is well worth your time.
When teaching people how to make graphics with data, however,
I have repeatedly found the need for an introduction that motivates
and explains why you are doing something but that does not skip
the necessary details of how to produce the images you see on the
page. And so this book has two main aims. First, I want you to get
to the point where you can reproduce almost every figure in the
text for yourself. Second, I want you to understand why the code
is written the way it is, such that when you look at data of your
own you can feel confident about your ability to get from a rough
picture in your head to a high-quality graphic on your screen or
page.
What You Will Learn
This book is a hands-on introduction to the principles and practice of looking at and presenting data using R and ggplot. R is a
powerful, widely used, and freely available programming language
for data analysis. You may be interested in exploring ggplot after
having used R before or be entirely new to both R and ggplot and
just want to graph your data. I do not assume you have any prior
knowledge of R.
After installing the software we need, we begin with an overview
ofsomebasicprinciplesofvisualization. Wefocusnotjustontheaesthetic aspects of good plots but on how their effectiveness is rooted
in the way we perceive properties like length, absolute and relative
size, orientation, shape, and color. We then learn how to produce
and refine plots using ggplot2, a powerful, versatile, and widely
used visualization package for R (Wickham 2016). The ggplot2
library implements a “grammar of graphics” (Wilkinson 2005).
This approach gives us a coherent way to produce visualizations
by expressing relationships between the attributes of data and their
graphical representation.
Through a series of worked examples, you will learn how to
build plots piece by piece, beginning with scatterplots and summaries of single variables, then moving on to more complex graphics. Topics covered include plotting continuous and categorical
Preface
variables; layering information on graphics; faceting grouped data
to produce effective “small multiple” plots; transforming data to
easily produce visual summaries on the graph such as trend lines,
linear fits, error ranges, and boxplots; creating maps; and some
alternatives to maps worth considering when presenting countryor state-level data. We will also cover cases where we are not
working directly with a dataset but rather with estimates from a
statistical model. From there, we will explore the process of refining plots to accomplish common tasks such as highlighting key
features of the data, labeling particular items of interest, annotating plots, and changing their overall appearance. Finally we
will examine some strategies for presenting graphical results in
different formats and to different sorts of audiences.
If you follow the text and examples in this book, then by the
end you will




understand the basic principles behind effective data visualization;
have a practical sense for why some graphs and figures work
well, while others may fail to inform or actively mislead;
know how to create a wide range of plots in R using ggplot2;
and
know how to refine plots for effective presentation.
Learning how to visualize data effectively is more than just
knowing how to write code that produces figures from data. This
book will teach you how to do that. But it will also teach you
how to think about the information you want to show, and how to
consider the audience you are showing it to—including the most
common case, when the audience is yourself.
This book is not a comprehensive guide to R, or even a comprehensive survey of everything ggplot can do. Nor is it a cookbook
containing just examples of specific things people commonly want
to do with ggplot. (Both these sorts of books already exist: see the
references in the appendix.) Neither is it a rigid set of rules, or a
sequence of beautiful finished examples that you can admire but
not reproduce. My goal is to get you quickly up and running in R,
making plots in a well-informed way, with a solid grasp of the core
sequence of steps—taking your data, specifying the relationship
between variables and visible elements, and building up images
layer by layer—that is at the heart of what ggplot does.

xiii
xiv

Preface
Learning ggplot does mean getting used to how R works, and
also understanding how ggplot connects to other tools in the R
language. As you work your way through the book, you will gradually learn more about some very useful idioms, functions, and
techniques for manipulating data in R. In particular you will learn
about some of the tools provided by the tidyverse library that
ggplot belongs to. Similarly, although this is not a cookbook, once
you get past chapter 1 you will be able to see and understand the
code used to produce almost every figure in the book. In most
cases you will also see these figures built up piece by piece, a step
at a time. If you use the book as it is designed, by the end you will
have the makings of a version of the book itself, containing code
you have written out and annotated yourself. And though we do
not go into great depth on the topic of rules or principles of visualization, the discussion in chapter 1 and its application throughout
the book gives you more to think about than just a list of graph
types. By the end of the book you should be able to look at a figure
and be able to see it in terms of ggplot’s grammar, understanding how the various layers, shapes, and data are pieced together to
make a finished plot.
The Right Frame of Mind
It can be a little disorienting to learn a programming language like
R, mostly because at the beginning there seem to be so many pieces
to fit together in order for things to work properly. It can seem like
you have to learn everything before you can do anything. The language has some possibly unfamiliar concepts that define how it
works, like “object,” “function,” or “class.” The syntactic rules for
writing code are annoyingly picky. Error messages seem obscure;
help pages are terse; other people seem to have had not quite the
same issue as you. Beyond that, you sense that doing one thing
often involves learning a bit about some other part of the language.
To make a plot you need a table of data, but maybe you need to
filter out some rows, recalculate some columns, or just get the computer to see it is there in the first place. And there is also a wider
environment of supporting applications and tools that are good to
know about but involve new concepts of their own—editors that
highlight what you write; applications that help you organize your
Preface

code and its output; ways of writing your code that let you keep
track of what you have done. It can all seem a bit confusing.
Don’t panic. You have to start somewhere. Starting with graphics is more rewarding than some of the other places you might
begin, because you will be able to see the results of your efforts very
quickly. As you build your confidence and ability in this area, you
will gradually see the other tools as things that help you sort out
some issue or solve a problem that’s stopping you from making the
picture you want. That makes them easier to learn. As you acquire
them piecemeal—perhaps initially using them without completely
understanding what is happening—you will begin to see how they
fit together and be more confident of your own ability to do what
you need to do.
Even better, in the past decade or so the world of data analysis and programming generally has opened up in a way that has
made help much easier to come by. Free tools for coding have been
around for a long time, but in recent years what we might call the
“ecology of assistance” has gotten better. There are more resources
available for learning the various pieces, and more of them are oriented to the way writing code actually happens most of the time—
which is to say, iteratively, in an error-prone fashion, and taking
account of problems other people have run into and solved before.
How to Use This Book
This book can be used in any one of several ways. At a minimum,
you can sit down and read it for a general overview of good practices in data visualization, together with many worked examples of
graphics from their beginnings to a properly finished state. Even
if you do not work through the code, you will get a good sense of
how to think about visualization and a better understanding of the
process through which good graphics are produced.
More useful, if you set things up as described in chapter 2 and
then work through the examples, you will end up with a data visualization book of your own. If you approach the book this way,
then by the end you will be comfortable using ggplot in particular
and also be ready to learn more about the R language in general.
This book can also be used to teach with, either as the main
focus of a course on data visualization or as a supplement to
You can also bring your own data to
explore instead of or alongside the
examples, as described in chapter 2.
xv
xvi

Preface
undergraduate or graduate courses in statistics or data analysis. My
aim has been to make the “hidden tasks” of coding and polishing
graphs more accessible and explicit. I want to make sure you are
not left with the “How to Draw an Owl in Three Steps” problem
common to many tutorials. You know the one. The first two steps
are shown clearly enough. Sketch a few bird-shaped ovals. Make
a line for a branch. But the final step, an owl such as John James
Audubon might have drawn, is presented as a simple extension for
readers to figure out for themselves.
If you have never used R or ggplot, you should start at the
beginning of the book and work your way through to the end.
If you know about R already and only want to learn the core of
ggplot, then after installing the software described below, focus on
chapters 3 through 5. Chapter 6 (on models) necessarily incorporates some material on statistical modeling that the book cannot
develop fully. This is not a statistics text. So, for example, I show
generally how to fit and work with various kinds of model in chapter 6, but I do not go through the important details of fitting,
selecting, and fully understanding different approaches. I provide
references in the text to other books that have this material as their
main focus.
Each chapter ends with a section suggesting where to go next
(apart from continuing to read the book). Sometimes I suggest
other books or websites to explore. I also ask questions or pose
some challenges that extend the material covered in the chapter,
encouraging you to use the concepts and skills you have learned.
Conventions
In this book we alternate between regular text (like this), samples
of code that you can type and run yourself, and the output of that
code. In the main text, references to objects or other things that
exist in the R language or in your R project—tables of data, variables, functions, and so on—will also appear in a monospaced or
“typewriter” typeface. Code you can type directly into R at the
console will be in gray boxes and also monospaced, like this:
my_numbers ←
Additional notes and information will sometimes
appear in the margin, like this.
c(1, 1, 4, 1, 1, 4, 1)
If you type that line of code into R’s console, it will create a
thing called my_numbers. Doing this doesn’t produce any output,
Preface
however. When we write code that also produces output at the console, we will first see the code (in a gray box) and then the output
in a monospaced font against a white background. Here we add
two numbers and see the result:
4 + 1
## [1] 5
Two further notes about how to read this. First, by default in
this book, anything that comes back to us at the console as the
result of typing a command will be shown prefaced by two hash
characters (##) at the beginning of each line of output. This is to
help distinguish it from commands we type into the console. You
will not see the hash characters at the console when you use R.
Second, both in the book and at the console, if the output of
what you did results in a series of elements (numbers, observations from a variable, and so on), you will often see output that
includes some number in square brackets at the beginning of the
line. It looks like this: [1]. This is not part of the output itself
but just a counter or index keeping track of how many items have
been printed out so far. In the case of adding 4 + 1 we got just
one, or [1], thing back—the number five. If there are more elements returned as the result of some instruction or command, the
counter will keep track of that on each line. In this next bit of code
we will tell R to show us the lowercase letters of the alphabet:
letters
## [1] “a” “b” “c” “d” “e” “f” “g” “h” “i” “j”
## [11] “k” “l” “m” “n” “o” “p” “q” “r” “s” “t”
## [21] “u” “v” “w” “x” “y” “z”
You can see the counter incrementing on each line as it keeps
count of how many letters have been printed.
Before You Begin
The book is designed for you to follow along in an active way, writing out the examples and experimenting with the code as you go.
You will be able to reproduce almost all the plots in the text. You
need to install some software first. Here is what to do:

xvii
xviii

Preface
1.
cloud.r-project.org
rstudio.com
tidyverse.org
I strongly recommend typing all the code examples
right from the beginning, instead of copying and
pasting.
Get the most recent version of R. It is free and available for
Windows, Mac, and Linux operating systems. Download the
version of R compatible with your operating system. If you are
running Windows or MacOS, choose one of the precompiled
binary distributions (i.e., ready-to-run applications) linked at
the top of the R Project’s web page.
2. Once R is installed, download and install R Studio, which is an
“Integrated Development Environment,” or IDE. This means
it is a front-end for R that makes it much easier to work with. R
Studio is also free and available for Windows, Mac, and Linux
platforms.
3. Install the tidyverse and several other add-on packages for R.
These packages provide useful functionality that we will take
advantage of throughout the book. You can learn more about
the tidyverse’s family of packages at its website.
To install the tidyverse, make sure you have an internet connection and then launch R Studio. Type the following lines of code
at R’s command prompt, located in the window named “Console,”
and hit return. In the code below, the ← arrow is made up of two
keystrokes, first < and then the short dash or minus symbol, -. my_packages ← c("tidyverse", "broom", "coefplot", "cowplot", "gapminder", "GGally", "ggrepel", "ggridges", "gridExtra", "here", "interplot", "margins", "maps", "mapproj", "mapdata", "MASS", "quantreg", "rlang", "scales", "survey", "srvyr", "viridis", "viridisLite", "devtools") install.packages(my_packages, repos = "http://cran.rstudio.com") github.com GitHub is a web-based service where users can host, develop, and share code. It uses git, a version control system that allows projects, or repositories, to preserve their history and incorporate changes from contributors in an organized way. R Studio should then download and install these packages for you. It may take a little while to download everything. With these packages available, you can then install one last library of material that’s useful specifically for this book. It is hosted on GitHub, rather than R’s central package repository, so we use a different function to fetch it. devtools::install_github("kjhealy/socviz") Once you’ve done that, we can get started. Data Visualization 1 Look at Data Some data visualizations are better than others. This chapter discusses why that is. While it is tempting to simply start laying down the law about what works and what doesn’t, the process of making a really good or really useful graph cannot be boiled down to a list of simple rules to be followed without exception in all circumstances. The graphs you make are meant to be looked at by someone. The effectiveness of any particular graph is not just a matter of how it looks in the abstract but also a question of who is looking at it, and why. An image intended for an audience of experts reading a professional journal may not be readily interpretable by the general public. A quick visualization of a dataset you are currently exploring might not be of much use to your peers or your students. Some graphs work well because they depend in part on some strong aesthetic judgments about what will be effective. That sort of good judgment is hard to systematize. However, data visualization is not simply a matter of competing standards of good taste. Some approaches work better for reasons that have less to do with one’s sense of what looks good and more to do with how human visual perception works. When starting out, it is easier to grasp these perceptual aspects of data visualization than it is to get a reliable, taste-based feel for what works. For this reason, it is better to begin by thinking about the relationship between the structure of your data and the perceptual features of your graphics. Getting into that habit will carry you a long way toward developing the ability to make good taste-based judgments, too. As we shall see later on, when working with data in R and ggplot, we get many visualization virtues for free. In general, the default layout and look of ggplot’s graphics is well chosen. This makes it easier to do the right thing. It also means that, if you really just want to learn how to make some plots right this minute, you could skip this chapter altogether and go straight to the next one. But although we will not be writing any code for the next few pages, we will be discussing aspects of graph construction, perception, and interpretation that matter for code you will choose to write. So 2 • Chapter 1 1 2 3 4 I urge you to stick around and follow the argument of this chapter. When making graphs there is only so much that your software can do to keep you on the right track. It cannot force you to be honest with yourself, your data, and your audience. The tools you use can help you live up to the right standards. But they cannot make you do the right thing. This means it makes sense to begin cultivating your own good sense about graphs right away. We will begin by asking why we should bother to look at pictures of data in the first place, instead of relying on tables or numerical summaries. Then we will discuss a few examples, first of bad visualization practice, and then more positively of work that looks (and is) much better. We will examine the usefulness and limits of general rules of thumb in visualization and show how even tasteful, well-constructed graphics can mislead us. From there we will briefly examine some of what we know about the perception of shapes, colors, and relationships between objects. The core point here is that we are quite literally able to see some things much more easily than others. These cognitive aspects of data visualization make some kinds of graphs reliably harder for people to interpret. Cognition and perception are relevant in other ways, too. We tend to make inferences about relationships between the objects that we see in ways that bear on our interpretation of graphical data, for example. Arrangements of points and lines on a page can encourage us—sometimes quite unconsciously—to make inferences about similarities, clustering, distinctions, and causal relationships that might or might not be there in the numbers. Sometimes these perceptual tendencies can be honestly harnessed to make our graphics more effective. At other times, they will tend to lead us astray, and we must take care not to lean on them too much. In short, good visualization methods offer extremely valuable tools that we should use in the process of exploring, understanding, and explaining data. But they are not a magical means of seeing the world as it really is. They will not stop you from trying to fool other people if that is what you want to do, and they may not stop you from fooling yourself either. 12.5 10.0 7.5 y values 5.0 12.5 10.0 7.5 1.1 Why Look at Data? 5.0 5 10 15 5 10 x values Figure 1.1: Plots of Anscombe’s quartet. 15 Anscombe’s quartet (Anscombe 1973; Chatterjee & Firat 2007), shown in figure 1.1, presents its argument for looking at data in visual form. It uses a series of four scatterplots. A scatterplot shows Look at Data Correlations can run from -1 to 1, with zero meaning there is no association. A score of -1 means a perfect negative association and a score of 1 a perfect positive asssociation between the two variables. So 0.81 counts as a strong positive correlation. 10 South Africa 9 8 Income inequality* the relationship between two quantities, such as height and weight, age and income, or time and unemployment. Scatterplots are the workhorse of data visualization in social science, and we will be looking at a lot of them. The data for Anscombe’s plots comes bundled with R. You can look at it by typing anscombe at the command prompt. Each of the four made-up “datasets” contains eleven observations of two variables, x and y. By construction, the numerical properties of each pair of x and y variables, such as their means, are almost identical. Moreover, the standard measures of the association between each x and y pair also match. The correlation coefficient is a strong 0.81 in every case. But when the datasets are visualized as a scatterplot, with the x variables plotted on the horizontal axis and the y variables on the vertical, the differences between them are readily apparent. Anscombe’s quartet is an extreme, manufactured example. But the benefits of visualizing one’s data can be shown in real cases. Figure 1.2 shows a graph from Jackman (1980), a short comment on Hewitt (1977). The original paper had argued for a significant association between voter turnout and income inequality based on a quantitative analysis of eighteen countries. When this relationship was graphed as a scatterplot, however, it immediately became clear that the quantitative association depended entirely on the inclusion of South Africa in the sample. An exercise by Jan Vanhove (2016) demonstrates the usefulness of looking at model fits and data at the same time. Figure 1.3 presents an array of scatterplots. As with Anscombe’s quartet, each panel shows the association between two variables. Within each panel, the correlation between the x and y variables is set to be 0.6, a pretty good degree of association. But the actual distribution of points is created by a different process in each case. In the top left panel each variable is normally distributed around its mean value. In other panels there is a single outlying point far off in one direction or another. Others are produced by more subtle rules. But each gives rise to the same basic linear association. Illustrations like this demonstrate why it is worth looking at data. But that does not mean that looking at data is all one needs to do. Real datasets are messy, and while displaying them graphically is very useful, doing so presents problems of its own. As we will see below, there is considerable debate about what sort of visual work is most effective, when it can be superfluous, and how it can at times be misleading to researchers and audiences alike. Just like seemingly sober and authoritative tables of numbers, data 3 • 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 0 20 40 60 80 100 % Turnout Key. Bivariate slope including South Africa (N = 18) Bivariate slope excluding South Africa (N = 17) Figure 1.2: Seeing the effect of an outlier on a regression line. A more careful quantitative approach could have found this issue as well, for example, with a proper sensitivity analysis. But the graphic makes the case directly. 1. Normal x, normal residuals 2. Uniform x, normal residuals 3. + skewed x, normal residuals 0.50 4. – skewed x, normal residuals 0.75 0.50 0.2 0.25 –0.2 –0.25 –0.4 –2 –1 0 1 0.00 0.25 0.0 0.00 0.25 0.00 –0.25 –0.25 –0.50 –0.50 2 0.25 5. Normal x, + skewed residuals 0.50 0.75 1.00 0 6. Normal x, - skewed residuals 250 500 750 4000 1000 7. Increasing spread 4500 5000 8. Decreasing spread 0.50 0.25 0.8 0.2 0.25 0.00 0.0 0.4 0.0 –2 –0.50 –0.25 –0.2 –2 2 1 0 0.00 –1 1 0 –0.4 0 1 2 3 4 5 0 2 1 3 4 5 y –1 –0.25 9. Quadratic trend 10. Sinusoid relationship 11. A single positive outlier 12. A single negative outlier 1.0 0.6 0.2 0.4 0.0 0.5 0.5 0.0 0.2 0.0 0.0 –0.2 –0.2 –0.4 –2 –1 0 1 2 –4 13. Bimodal residuals 0 4 –2.5 14. Two groups 0.0 2.5 5.0 7.5 10.0 0 15. Sampling at the extremes 4 8 16. Categorical data 0.4 0.4 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 –0.2 –0.2 –0.2 –0.2 0.2 –0.4 –0.4 –2 –1 0 1 –5.0 –2.5 0.0 2.5 5.0 –4 0 4 8 1 2 3 4 5 x Figure 1.3: What data patterns can lie behind a correlation? The correlation coefficient in all these plots is 0.6. Figure adapted from code by Jan Vanhove. Look at Data visualizations have their own rhetoric of plausibility. Anscombe’s quartet notwithstanding, and especially for large volumes of data, summary statistics and model estimates should be thought of as tools that we use to deliberately simplify things in a way that lets us see past a cloud of data points shown in a figure. We will not automatically get the right answer to our questions just by looking. 1.2 What Makes Bad Figures Bad? It is traditional to begin discussions of data visualization with a “parade of horribles,” in an effort to motivate good behavior later. However, these negative examples often combine several kinds of badness that are better kept separate. For convenience, we can say that our problems tend to come in three varieties. Some are strictly aesthetic. The graph we are looking at is in some way tacky, tasteless, or a hodgepodge of ugly or inconsistent design choices. Some are substantive. Here, our graph has problems that are due to the data being presented. Good taste might make things look better, but what we really need is to make better use of the data we have, or get new information and plot that instead. And some problems are perceptual. In these cases, even with good aesthetic qualities and good data, the graph will be confusing or misleading because of how people perceive and process what they are looking at. It is important to understand that these elements, while often found together, are distinct from one another. Bad taste Let’s start with the bad taste. The chart in figure 1.4 both is tasteless and has far too much going on in it, given the modest amount of information it displays. The bars are hard to read and compare. It needlessly duplicates labels and makes pointless use of three-dimensional effects, drop shadows, and other unnecessary design features. The best-known critic by far of this style of visualization, and the best-known taste-maker in the field, is Edward R. Tufte. His book The Visual Display of Quantitative Information (1983) is a classic, and its sequels are also widely read (Tufte 1990, 1997). The • 5 6 • Chapter 1 Figure 1.4: A chart with a considerable amount of junk in it. bulk of this work is a series of examples of good and bad visualization, along with some articulation of more general principles (or rules of thumb) extracted from them. It is more like a reference book about completed dishes than a cookbook for daily use in the kitchen. At the same time, Tufte’s early academic work in political science shows that he effectively applied his own ideas to research questions. His Political Control of the Economy (1978) combines tables, figures, and text in a manner that remains remarkably fresh almost forty years later. Tufte’s message is sometimes frustrating, but it is consistent: Graphical excellence is the well-designed presentation of interesting data—a matter of substance, of statistics, and of design. . . . [It] consists of complex ideas communicated with clarity, precision, and efficiency. . . . [It] is that which gives to the viewer the greatest number of ideas in the shortest time with the least ink in the smallest space. . . . [It] is nearly always multivariate. . . . And graphical excellence requires telling the truth about the data. (Tufte 1983, 51) Tufte illustrates the point with Charles Joseph Minard’s famous visualization of Napoleon’s march on Moscow, shown here in Look at Data • 7 Figure 1.5: Minard’s visualization of Napoleon’s retreat from Moscow. Justifiably cited as a classic, it is also atypical and hard to emulate in its specifics. figure 1.5. He remarks that this image “may well be the best statistical graphic ever drawn” and argues that it “tells a rich, coherent story with its multivariate data, far more enlightening than just a single number bouncing along over time. Six variables are plotted: the size of the army, its location on a two-dimensional surface, direction of the army’s movement, and temperature on various dates during the retreat from Moscow.” It is worth noting how far removed Minard’s image is from most contemporary statistical graphics. At least until recently, these have tended to be applications or generalizations of scatterplots and bar plots, in the direction of either seeing more raw data or seeing the output derived from a statistical model. The former looks for ways to increase the volume of data visible, or the number of variables displayed within a panel, or the number of panels displayed within a plot. The latter looks for ways to see results such as point estimates, confidence intervals, and predicted probabilities in an easily comprehensible way. Tufte acknowledges that a tour de force such as Minard’s “can be described and admired, but there are no compositional principles on how to create that one wonderful graphic in a million.” The best one can do for “more routine, workaday designs” is to suggest some guidelines such as “have a properly chosen format and design,” “use words, numbers, and drawing together,” “display an accessible complexity of detail,” and “avoid content-free decoration, including chartjunk” (Tufte 1983, 177). In practice those compositional principles have amounted to an encouragement to maximize the “data-to-ink” ratio. This is practical advice. It is not hard to jettison tasteless junk, and if 8 • Chapter 1 Figure 1.6: “Monstrous Costs” by Nigel Holmes (1982). Also a classic of its kind. a b c d e f Figure 1.7: Six kinds of summary boxplots. Type (c) is from Tufte. we look a little harder we may find that the chart can do without other visual scaffolding as well. We can often clean up the typeface, remove extraneous colors and backgrounds, and simplify, mute, or delete gridlines, superfluous axis marks, or needless keys and legends. Given all that, we might think that a solid rule of “simpify, simplify” is almost all of what we need to make sure that our charts remain junk-free and thus effective. Unfortunately this is not the case. For one thing, somewhat annoyingly, there is evidence that highly embellished charts like Nigel Holmes’s “Monstrous Costs” (fig. 1.6) are often more easily recalled than their plainer alternatives (Bateman et al. 2010). Viewers do not find them more easily interpretable, but they do remember them more easily and also seem to find them more enjoyable to look at. They also associate them more directly with value judgments, as opposed to just trying to get information across. Borkin et al. (2013) also found that visually unique, “infographic”-style graphs were more memorable than more standard statistical visualizations. (“It appears that novel and unexpected visualizations can be better remembered than the visualizations with limited variability that we are exposed to since elementary school,” they remark.) Even worse, it may be the case that graphics that really do maximize the data-to-ink ratio are harder to interpret than those that are a little more relaxed about it. E. W. Anderson et al. (2011) found that, of the six kinds of boxplot shown in figure 1.7, the minimalist version from Tufte’s own work (option C) proved to be the most cognitively difficult for viewers to interpret. Cues like labels and gridlines, together with some strictly superfluous embellishment of data points or other design elements, may often be an aid rather than an impediment to interpretation. While chartjunk is not entirely devoid of merit, bear in mind that ease of recall is only one virtue among many for graphics. It is also the case that, almost by definition, it is no easier to systematize the construction of a chart like “Monstrous Costs” than it is to replicate the impact of Minard’s graph of Napoleon’s retreat. Indeed, the literature on chartjunk suggests that the two may have some qualities in common. To be sure, Minard’s figure is admirably rich in data while Holmes’s is not. But both are visually distinctive in a way that makes them memorable, both show a substantial amount of bespoke design, and both are unlike most of the statistical graphs you will see or make. Look at Data • 9 Bad data In your everyday work you will be in little danger of producing either a “Monstrous Costs” or a “Napoleon’s Retreat.” You are much more likely to make a good-looking, well-designed figure that misleads people because you have used it to display some bad data. Well-designed figures with little or no junk in their component parts are not by themselves a defense against cherry-picking your data or presenting information in a misleading way. Indeed, it is even possible that, in a world where people are on guard against junky infographics, the “halo effect” accompanying a wellproduced figure might make it easier to mislead some audiences. Or, perhaps more common, good aesthetics does not make it much harder for you to mislead yourself as you look at your data. In November 2016 the New York Times reported on some research on people’s confidence in the institutions of democracy. It had been published in an academic journal by the political scientists Yascha Mounk and Roberto Stefan Foa. The headline in the Times ran “How Stable Are Democracies? ‘Warning Signs Are Flashing Red’ ” (Taub 2016). The graph accompanying the article, reproduced in figure 1.8, certainly seemed to show an alarming decline. The graph was widely circulated on social media. It is impressively well produced. It’s an elegant small-multiple that, in addition to the point ranges it identifies, also shows an error range (labeled as such for people who might not know what it is), and the story told across the panels for each country is pretty consistent. Percentage of people who say it is “essential” to live in a democracy Sweden Australia Netherlands United States New Zealand Britain 100% Figure 1.8: A crisis of faith in democracy? (Source: Roberto Stefan Foa and Yascha Mounk, “The Signs of Deconsolidation,” Journal of Democracy, 28(1), 5–16.) 75% 50% 95% confidence intervals 25% 1930s 1980s ‘30s ‘80s ‘30s ‘80s ‘30s Decade of birth ‘80s ‘30s ‘80s ‘30s ‘80s 10 • Chapter 1 One reason I chose this example is that, at the time of writing, it is not unreasonable to be concerned about the stability of people’s commitment to democratic government in some Western countries. Perhaps Mounk’s argument is correct. But in such cases, the question is how much we are letting the data speak to us, as opposed to arranging it to say what we already think for other reasons. The figure is a little tricky to interpret. As the x-axis label says, the underlying data are from a cross-sectional survey of people of different ages rather than a longitudinal study measuring everyone at different times. Thus the lines do not show a trend measured each decade from the 1930s but rather differences in the answers given by people born in different decades, all of whom were asked the question at the same time. Given that, a bar graph might have been a more appropriate to display the results. More important, as the story circulated, helped by the compelling graphic, scholars who knew the World Values Survey data underlying the graph noticed something else. The graph reads as though people were asked to say whether they thought it was essential to live in a democracy, and the results plotted show the percentage of respondents who said “Yes,” presumably in contrast to those who said “No.” But in fact the survey question asked respondents to rate the importance of living in a democracy on a ten-point scale, with 1 being “Not at all Important” and 10 being “Absolutely Important.” The graph showed the difference across ages of people who had given a score of 10 only, not changes in the average score on the question. As it turns out, while there is some variation by year of birth, most people in these countries tend to rate the importance of living in a democracy very highly, even if they do not all score it as “Absolutely Important.” The political scientist Erik Voeten redrew the figure using the average response. The results are shown in figure 1.9. Australia Netherlands Sweden Great Britain United States Figure 1.9: Perhaps the crisis has been overblown. (Erik Voeten.) Average importance of democracy 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 30s 50s 70s 90s 30s 50s 70s 90s 30s 50s 70s 90s 30s 50s 70s 90s 30s 50s 70s 90s Decade of birth Graph by Erik Voeten, based on WVS 5 Look at Data • 11 The change here is not due to a difference in how the y-axis is drawn. That is a common issue with graphs, and one we will discuss below. In this case both the New York Times graph and Voeten’s alternative have scales that cover the full range of possible values (from 0 to 100% in the former case and from 1 to 10 in the latter). Rather, a different measure is being shown. We are now looking at the trend in the average score, rather than the trend for the highest possible answer. Substantively, there does still seem to be a decline in the average score by age cohort, on the order of between 0.5 point and 1.5 points on a 10-point scale. It could be an early warning sign of a collapse of belief in democracy, or it could be explained by something else. It might even be reasonable (as we will see for a different example shortly) to present the data in Voeten’s version with the y-axis covering just the range of the decline, rather than the full 0–10 scale. But it seems fair to say that the story might not have made the New York Times if the original research article had presented Voeten’s version of the data rather than the one that appeared in the newspaper. Bad perception Our third category of badness lives in the gap between data and aesthetics. Visualizations encode numbers in lines, shapes, and colors. That means that our interpretation of these encodings is partly conditional on how we perceive geometric shapes and relationships generally. We have known for a long time that poorly encoded data can be misleading. Tufte (1983) contains many examples, as does Wainer (1984). Many of the instances they cite revolve around needlessly multiplying the number of dimensions shown in a plot. Using an area to represent a length, for example, can make differences between observations look larger than they are. Although the most egregious abuses are less common than they once were, adding additional dimensions to plots remains a common temptation. Figure 1.10, for instance, is a 3-D bar chart made using a recent version of Microsoft Excel. Charts like this are common in business presentations and popular journalism and are also seen in academic journal articles from time to time. Here we seek to avoid too much junk by using Excel’s default settings. As To be fair, the 3-D format is not Excel’s default type of bar chart. 12 • Chapter 1 Figure 1.10: A 3-D column chart created in Microsoft Excel for Mac. Although it may seem hard to believe, the values shown in the bars are 1, 2, 3, and 4. A B C D 1 2 3 4 3D column charts in excel are awesome 4 3 2 1 0 A B C D you can see from the cells shown to the left of the chart, the data we are trying to plot is not very complex. The chart even tries to help us by drawing and labeling grid lines on the y- (and z-) axes. And yet the 3-D columns in combination with the default angle of view for the chart make the values as displayed differ substantially from the ones actually in the cell. Each column appears to be somewhat below its actual value. It is possible to see, if you squint with your mind’s eye, how the columns would line up with the axis guidelines if your angle of view moved so that the bars were head-on. But as it stands, anyone asked what values the chart shows would give the wrong answer. By now, many regular users of statistical graphics know enough to avoid excessive decorative embellishments of charts. They are also usually put on their guard by overly elaborate presentation of simple trends, as when a three-dimensional ribbon is used to display a simple line. Moreover, the default settings of most current graphical software tend to make the user work a little harder to add these features to plots. Even when the underlying numbers are sensible, the default settings of software are good, and the presentation of charts is mostly junk-free, some charts remain more difficult to interpret than others. They encode data in ways that are hard for viewers to understand. Figure 1.11 presents a stacked bar chart with time in years on the x-axis and some value on the y-axis. The bars show the total value, with subdivisions by the relative contribution of different categories to each year’s observation. Charts like this are common when showing the absolute contribution of various products to total sales over time, for example, or the number of different groups of people in a changing population. Equivalently, stacked Look at Data Type A B C 13 D Figure 1.11: A junk-free plot that remains hard to interpret. While a stacked bar chart makes the overall trend clear, it can make it harder to see the trends for the categories within the bar. This is partly due to the nature of the trends. But if the additional data is hard to understand, perhaps it should not be included to begin with. 2000 1500 Value • 1000 500 0 2004 2007 2010 2013 2016 Year 4000 Figure 1.12: Aspect ratios affect our perception of rates of change. (After an example by William S. Cleveland.) 4000 2000 Value Value 3000 1000 3000 2000 1000 0 0 0 2 4 x 6 8 0 2 4 x 6 8 line-graphs showing similar kinds of trends are also common for data with many observation points on the x-axis, such as quarterly observations over a decade. In a chart like this, the overall trend is readily interpretable, and it is also possible to easily follow the over-time pattern of the category that is closest to the x-axis baseline (in this case, type D, in purple). But the fortunes of the other categories are not so easily grasped. Comparisons of both the absolute and the relative share of type B or C are much more difficult, whether one wants to compare trends within type or between them. Relative comparisons need a stable baseline. In this case, that’s the x-axis, which is why the overall trend and the type D trend are much easier to see than any other trend. A different sort of problem is shown in figure 1.12. In the left panel, the lines appear at first glance to be converging as the value of x increases. It seems like they might even intersect if we extended the graph out further. In the right panel, the curves are clearly equidistant from the beginning. The data plotted in each panel is the same, however. The apparent convergence in the left panel is just a result of the aspect ratio of the figure. 14 • Chapter 1 These problems are not easily solved by the application of good taste, or by following a general rule to maximize the data-to-ink ratio, even though that is a good rule to follow. Instead, we need to know a little more about the role of perception in the interpretation of graphs. Fortunately for us, this is an area that has produced a substantial amount of research over the past twenty-five years. 1.3 Perception and Data Visualization While a detailed discussion of visual perception is well beyond the scope of this book, even a simple sense of how we see things will help us understand why some figures work and others do not. For a much more thorough treatment of these topics, Colin Ware’s books on information design are excellent overviews of research on visual perception, written from the perspective of people designing graphs, figures, and systems for representing data (Ware 2008, 2013). Edges, contrasts, and colors Figure 1.13: Hermann grid effect. Looking at pictures of data means looking at lines, shapes, and colors. Our visual system works in a way that makes some things easier for us to see than others. I am speaking in slightly vague terms here because the underlying details are the remit of vision science, and the exact mechanisms responsible are often the subject of ongoing research. I will not pretend to summarize or evaluate this material. In any case, independent of detailed explanation, the existence of the perceptual phenomena themselves can often be directly demonstrated through visual effects or “optical illusions” of various kinds. These effects demonstrate that perception is not a simple matter of direct visual inputs producing straightforward mental representations of their content. Rather, our visual system is tuned to accomplish some tasks very well, and this comes at a cost in other ways. The active nature of perception has long been recognized. The Hermann grid effect, shown in figure 1.13, was discovered in 1870. Ghostly blobs seem to appear at the intersections in the grid but only as long as one is not looking at them directly. A related effect is shown in figure 1.14. These are Mach bands. When the gray bars share a boundary, the apparent contrast between them appears to Look at Data • 15 Figure 1.14: Mach bands. On the left side, five gray bars are ordered from dark to light, with gaps between them. On the right side, the bars have no gap between them. The brightness or luminance of the corresponding bars is the same. However, when the bars touch, the dark areas seem darker and the light areas lighter. increase. Speaking loosely, we can say that our visual system is trying to construct a representation of what it is looking at based more on relative differences in the luminance (or brightness) of the bars rather than on their absolute value. Similarly, the ghostly blobs in the Hermann grid effect can be thought of as a side-effect of the visual system being tuned for a different task. These sorts of effects extend to the role of background contrasts. The same shade of gray will be perceived differently depending on whether it is against a dark background or a light one. Our ability to distinguish shades of brightness is not uniform either. We are better at distinguishing dark shades than we are at distinguishing light ones. The effects interact, too. We will do better at distinguishing very light shades of gray when they are set against a light background. When set against a dark background, differences in the middle range of the light-to-dark spectrum are easier to distinguish. Our visual system is attracted to edges, and we assess contrast and brightness in terms of relative rather than absolute values. Some of the more spectacular visual effects exploit our mostly successful efforts to construct representations of surfaces, shapes, and objects based on what we are seeing. Edward Adelson’s checkershadow illusion, shown in figure 1.15, is a good example. Though hard to believe, the squares marked “A” and “B” are the same shade of gray. To figure out the shade of the squares on the floor, we compare it to the nearby squares, and we also discount the shadows cast by other objects. Even though a light-colored surface in shadow might reflect less light than a dark surface in direct light, it would generally be an error to infer that the surface in the shade really was a darker color. The checkerboard image is carefully constructed to exploit these visual inferences made based on local contrasts in brightness and the information provided by shadows. As Adelson (1995) notes, “The visual system is not very good at being a physical light meter, but that is not its purpose.” Because it has 16 • Chapter 1 Figure 1.15: The checkershadow illusion (Edward H. Adelson). Figure 1.16: Edge contrasts in monochrome and color, after Ware (2008). evolved to be good at perceiving real objects in its environment, we need to be aware of how it works in settings where we are using it to do other things, such as keying variables to some spectrum of grayscale values. An important point about visual effects of this kind is that they are not illusions in the way that a magic trick is an illusion. If a magician takes you through an illusion step by step and shows you how it is accomplished, then the next time you watch the trick performed you will see through it and notice the various bits of misdirection and sleight of hand that are used to achieve the effect. But the most interesting visual effects are not like this. Even after they have been explained to you, you cannot stop seeing them, because the perceptual processes they exploit are not under your conscious control. This makes it easy to be misled by them, as when (for example) we overestimate the size of a contrast between two adjacent shaded areas on a map or grid simply because they share a boundary. Our ability to see edge contrasts is stronger for monochrome images than for color. Figure 1.16, from Ware (2008, 71), shows an image of dunes. In the red-green version, the structure of the landscape is hard to perceive. In the grayscale version, the dunes and ridges are much more easily visible. Using color in data visualization introduces a number of other complications (Zeileis & Hornik 2006). The central one is related to the relativity of luminance perception. As we have been discussing, our perception of how bright something looks is largely a matter of relative rather than absolute judgments. How bright a surface looks depends partly on the brightness of objects near it. In addition to luminance, the color of an object can be thought of has having two other components. First, an object’s hue is what we conventionally mean when we use the word “color”: red, blue, Look at Data green, purple, and so on. In physical terms it can be thought of as the dominant wavelength of the light reflected from the object’s surface. The second component is chrominance or chroma. This is the intensity or vividness of the color. To produce color output on screens or in print we use various color models that mix together color components to get specific outputs. Using the RGB model, a computer might represent color in terms of mixtures of red, green, and blue components, each of which can take a range of values from 0 to 255. When using colors in a graph, we are mapping some quantity or category in our data to a color that people see. We want that mapping to be “accurate” in some sense, with respect to the data. This is partly a matter of the mapping being correct in strictly numerical terms. For instance, we want the gap between two numerical values in the data to be meaningfully preserved in the numerical values used to define the colors shown. But it is also partly a matter of how that mapping will be perceived when we look at the graph. For example, imagine we had a variable that could take values from 0 to 5 in increments of 1, with zero being the lowest value. It is straightforward to map this variable to a set of RGB colors that are equally distant from one another in purely numerical terms in our color space. The wrinkle is that many points that are equidistant from each other in this sense will not be perceived as equally distant by people looking at the graph. This is because our perception is not uniform across the space of possible colors. For instance, the range of chroma we are able to see depends strongly on luminance. If we pick the wrong color palette to represent our data, for any particular gradient the same-sized jump between one value and another (e.g., from 0 to 1, as compared to from 3 to 4) might be perceived differently by the viewer. This also varies across colors, in that numerically equal gaps between a sequences of reds (say) are perceived differently from the same gaps mapped to blues. When choosing color schemes, we will want mappings from data to color that are not just numerically but also perceptually uniform. R provides color models and color spaces that try to achieve this. Figure 1.17 shows a series of sequential gradients using the HCL (hue-chroma-luminance) color model. The grayscale gradient at the top varies by luminance only. The blue palette varies by luminance and chrominance, as the brightness and the intensity of the color vary across the spectrum. The remaining three palettes • 17 Sequential grayscale Sequential blue to gray Sequential terrain Diverging Unordered hues Figure 1.17: Five palettes generated from R’s color space library. From top to bottom, the sequential grayscale palette varies only in luminance, or brightness. The sequential blue palette varies in both luminance and chrominance (or intensity). The third sequential palette varies in luminance, chrominance, and hue. The fourth palette is diverging, with a neutral midpoint. The fifth features balanced hues, suitable for unordered categories. 18 • Chapter 1 The body responsible for this is the appropriately authoritative-sounding Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage, or International Commission on Illumination. vary by luminance, chrominance, and hue. The goal in each case is to generate a perceptually uniform scheme, where hops from one level to the next are seen as having the same magnitude. Gradients or sequential scales from low to high are one of three sorts of color palettes. When we are representing a scale with a neutral midpoint (as when we are showing temperatures, for instance, or variance in either direction from a zero point or a mean value), we want a diverging scale, where the steps away from the midpoint are perceptually even in both directions. The blue-tored palette in figure 1.17 displays an example. Finally, perceptual uniformity matters for unordered categorical variables as well. We often use color to represent data for different countries, or political parties, or types of people, and so on. In those cases we want the colors in our qualitative palette to be easily distinguishable but also have the same valence for the viewer. Unless we are doing it deliberately, we do not want one color to perceptually dominate the others. The bottom palette in figure 1.17 shows an example of a qualitative palette that is perceptually uniform in this way. The upshot is that we should generally not pick colors in an ad hoc way. It is too easy to go astray. In addition to the considerations we have been discussing, we also want to avoid producing plots that confuse people who are color-blind, for example. Fortunately, almost all the work has been done for us already. Different color spaces have been defined and standardized in ways that account for these uneven or nonlinear aspects of human color perception. R and ggplot make these features available to us for free. The default palettes we will be using in ggplot are perceptually uniform in the right way. If we want to get more adventurous later, the tools are available to produce custom palettes that still have desirable perceptual qualities. Our decisions about color will focus more on when and how it should be used. As we are about to see, color is a powerful channel for picking out visual elements of interest. Preattentive search and what “pops” Some objects in our visual field are easier to see than others. They pop out at us from whatever they are surrounded by. For some kinds of object, or through particular channels, this can happen very quickly. Indeed, from our point of view it happens before or Look at Data Color only, N = 20 Color only, N = 100 Shape only, N = 20 Shape only, N = 100 Figure 1.18: Searching for the blue circle becomes progressively harder. almost before the conscious act of looking at or for something. The general term for this is “preattentive pop-out,” and there is an extensive experimental and theoretical literature on it in psychology and vision science. As with the other perceptual processes we have been discussing, the explanation for what is happening is or has been a matter of debate, up to and including the degree to which the phenomenon really is “preattentive,” as discussed, for example, by Treisman & Gormican (1988) or Nakayama & Joseph (1998). But it is the existence of pop-out that is relevant to us, rather than its explanation. Pop-out makes some things on a data graphic easier to see or find than others. Consider the panels in figure 1.18. Each one of them contains a single blue circle. Think of it as an observation of interest. Reading left to right, the first panel contains twenty circles, nineteen of which are yellow and one blue. The blue circle is easy to find, as there are a relatively small number of observations to scan, and their color is the only thing that varies. The viewer barely has to search consciously at all before seeing the dot of interest. In the second panel, the search is harder, but not that much harder. There are a hundred dots now, five times as many, but again the blue dot is easily found. The third panel again has only twenty observations. But this time there is no variation on color. Instead nineteen observations are triangles and one is a circle. On average, looking for the blue dot is noticeably harder than searching for it in the first panel, and it may even be more difficult than in the second panel despite there being many fewer observations. Think of shape and color as two distinct channels that can be used to encode information visually. It seems that pop-out on the • 19 Color & shape, N = 100 20 • Chapter 1 13 13 12 12 11 11 Biscuits Biscuits color channel is stronger than it is on the shape channel. In the fourth panel, the number of observations is again upped to one hundred. Finding the single blue dot may take noticeably longer. If you don’t see it on the first or second pass, it may require a conscious effort to systematically scan the area in order to find it. It seems that search performance on the shape channel degrades much faster than on the color channel. Finally the fifth panel mixes color and shape for a large number of observations. Again there is only one blue dot on the graph, but annoyingly there are many blue triangles and yellow dots that make it harder to find what we are looking for. Dual- or multiplechannel searches for large numbers of observations can be very slow. Similar effects can be demonstrated for search across other channels (for instance, with size, angle, elongation, and movement) and for particular kinds of searches within channels. For example, some kinds of angle contrasts are easier to see than others, as are some kinds of color contrasts. Ware (2008, 27–33) has more discussion and examples. The consequences for data visualization are clear enough. As shown in figure 1.19, adding multiple channels to a graph is likely to quickly overtax the capacity of the viewer. Even if our software allows us to, we should think carefully before representing different variables and their values by shape, color, and position all at once. It is possible for there to be exceptions, in particular (as shown in the second panel of 10 10 9 9 8 8 7 7 8 9 10 Tea 11 12 8 9 10 Tea Figure 1.19: Multiple channels become uninterpretable very fast (left), unless your data has a great deal of structure (right). 11 12 Look at Data • 21 figure 1.19) if the data shows a great deal of structure to begin with. But even here, in all but the most straightforward cases a different visualization strategy is likely to do better. Gestalt rules At first glance, the points in the pop-out examples in figure 1.18 might seem randomly distributed within each panel. In fact, they are not quite randomly located. Instead, I wrote a little code to lay them out in a way that spread them around the plotting area but prevented any two points from completely or partially overlapping each other. I did this because I wanted the scatterplots to be programmatically generated but did not want to take the risk that the blue dot would end up plotted underneath one of the other dots or triangles. It’s worth taking a closer look at this case, as there is a lesson here for how we perceive patterns. Each panel in figure 1.20 shows a field of points. There are clearly differences in structure between them. The first panel was produced by a two-dimensional Poisson point process and is “properly” random. (Defining randomness, or ensuring that a process really is random, turns out to be a lot harder than you might think. But we gloss over those difficulties here.) The second panel was produced from a Matérn model, a specification often found in spatial statistics and ecology. In a model like this points are again randomly distributed but are subject to some local constraints. In this case, after randomly generating a number of candidate points in order, the field is pruned to eliminate any point that appears too close to a point that was generated before it. We can tune the model to decide how close is “too close.” The result is a set of points that are evenly spread across the available space. If you ask people which of these panels has more structure in it, they will tend to say the Poisson field. We associate randomness with a relatively even distribution across a space. But in fact, a random process like this is substantially more clumpy than we tend to think. I first saw a picture of this contrast in an essay by Stephen Jay Gould (1991). There the Matérn-like model was used as a representation of glowworms on the wall of a cave in New Zealand. It’s a good model for that case because if one glowworm gets too close to another, it’s liable to get eaten. Hence the relatively even—but not random—distribution that results. Poisson Matérn Figure 1.20: Each panel shows simulated data. The upper panel shows a random point pattern generated by a Poisson process. The lower panel is from a Matérn model, where new points are randomly placed but cannot be too near alreadyexisting ones. Most people see the Poissongenerated pattern as having more structure, or less “randomness,” than the Matérn, whereas the reverse is true. 22 • Chapter 1 Figure 1.21: Gestalt inferences: proximity, similarity, connection, common fate. The layout of the figure employs some of these principles, in addition to displaying them. We look for structure all the time. We are so good at it that we will find it in random data, given time. (This is one of the reasons that data visualization can hardly be a replacement for statistical modeling.) The strong inferences we make about relationships between visual elements from relatively sparse visual information are called “gestalt rules.” They are not pure perceptual effects like the checkerboard illusions. Rather, they describe our tendency to infer relationships between the objects we are looking at in a way that goes beyond what is strictly visible. Figure 1.21 provides some examples. What sorts of relationships are inferred, and under what circumstances? In general we want to identify groupings, classifications, or entities than can be treated as the same thing or part of the same thing: • • • • • • • Proximity: Things that are spatially near to one another seem to be related. Similarity: Things that look alike seem to be related. Connection: Things that are visually tied to one another seem to be related. Continuity: Partially hidden objects are completed into familiar shapes. Closure: Incomplete shapes are perceived as complete. Figure and ground: Visual elements are taken to be either in the foreground or in the background. Common fate: Elements sharing a direction of movement are perceived as a unit. Look at Data Some kinds of visual cues outweigh others. For example, in the upper left of figure 1.21, the circles are aligned horizontally into rows, but their proximity by column takes priority, and we see three groups of circles. In the upper right, the three groups are still salient but the row of blue circles is now seen as a grouped entity. In the middle row of the figure, the left side shows mixed grouping by shape, size, and color. Meanwhile the right side of the row shows that direct connection outweighs shape. Finally the two schematic plots in the bottom row illustrate both connection and common fate, in that the lines joining the shapes tend to be read left-toright as part of a series. Note also the points in the lower right plot where the lines cross. There are gaps in the line segments joining the circles, but we perceive this as them “passing underneath” the lines joining the triangles. 1.4 Visual Tasks and Decoding Graphs The workings of our visual system and our tendency to make inferences about relationships between visible elements form the basis of our ability to interpret graphs of data. There is more involved besides that, however. Beyond core matters of perception lies the question of interpreting and understanding particular kinds of graphs. The proportion of people who can read and correctly interpret a scatterplot is lower than you might think. At the intersection of perception and interpretation there are specific visual tasks that people need to perform in order to properly see the graph in front of them. To understand a scatterplot, for example, the viewer needs to know a lot of general information, such as what a variable is, what the x-y coordinate plane looks like, why we might want to compare two variables on it, and the convention of putting the supposed cause or “independent” variable on the x-axis. Even if viewers understand all these things, they must still perform the visual task of interpreting the graph. A scatterplot is a visual representation of data, not a way to magically transmit pure understanding. Even well-informed viewers may do worse than we think when connecting the picture to the underlying data (Doherty, et al. 2007; Rensink & Baldridge 2010). In the 1980s William S. Cleveland and Robert McGill conducted some experiments identifying and ranking theses tasks for different types of graphics (Cleveland & McGill, 1984, 1987). Most • 23 24 • Chapter 1 100 0 1 2 Position 3 4 5 Length 6 Angle 7 Circular area 8 9 Rectangular area Figure 1.22: Schematic representation of basic perceptual tasks for nine chart types, by Heer and Bostock, following Cleveland and McGill. In both studies, participants were asked to make comparisons of highlighted portions of each chart type and say which was smaller. often, research subjects were asked to estimate two values within a chart (e.g., two bars in a bar chart, or two slices of a pie chart) or compare values between charts (e.g., two areas in adjacent stacked bar charts). Cleveland went on to apply the results of this work, developing the trellis display system for data visualization in S, the statistical programming language developed at Bell Labs. (R is a later implementation of S.) He also wrote two excellent books that describe and apply these principles (Cleveland 1993, 1994). In 2010 Heer & Bostock replicated Cleveland’s earlier experiments and added a few assessments, including evaluations of rectangular-area graphs, which have become more popular in recent years. These include treemaps, where a square or rectangle is subdivided into further rectangular areas representing some proportion or percentage of the total. It looks a little like a stacked bar chart with more than one column. The comparisons and graph types made by their research subjects are shown schematically in figure 1.22. For each graph type, subjects were asked to identify the smaller of two marked segments on the chart and then to “make a quick visual judgment” estimating what percentage the smaller one was of the larger. As can be seen from the figure, the charts tested encoded data in different ways. Types 1–3 use position encoding along a common scale while types 4 and 5 use length encoding. The pie chart encodes values as angles, and the remaining charts as areas, using either circular, separate rectangles (as in a cartogram) or subrectangles (as in a treemap). Their results are shown in figure 1.23, along with Cleveland and McGill’s original results for comparison. The replication was quite good. The overall pattern of results seems clear, with performance worsening substantially as we move away from comparison on a common scale to length-based comparisons to angles and finally areas. Area comparisons perform even worse than the (justifiably) maligned pie chart. Look at Data • 25 Cleveland & McGill’s results T1 Figure 1.23: Cleveland and McGill’s original results (top) and Heer and Bostock’s replication with additions (bottom) for nine chart types. T2 T3 T4 T5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 2.5 3.0 Log error Crowdsourced results T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 T7 T8 T9 1.0 1.5 2.0 Log error These findings, and other work in this tradition, strongly suggest that there are better and worse ways of visually representing data when the task the user must perform involves estimating and comparing values within the graph. Think of this as a “decoding” operation that the viewer must perform in order to understand the content. The data values were encoded or mapped in to the graph, and now we have to get them back out again. When doing this, we do best judging the relative position of elements aligned on a common scale, as, for example, when we compare the heights of bars on a bar chart, or the position of dots with reference to a fixed x- or y-axis. When elements are not aligned but still share a scale, comparison is a little harder but still pretty good. It is more difficult again to compare the lengths of lines without a common baseline. Outside of position and length encodings, things generally become harder and the decoding process is more error prone. We tend to misjudge quantities encoded as angles. The size of acute angles tends to be underestimated, and the size of obtuse angles 26 • Chapter 1 overestimated. This is one reason pie charts are usually a bad idea. We also misjudge areas poorly. We have known for a long time that area-based comparisons of quantities are easily misinterpreted or exaggerated. For example, values in the data might be encoded as lengths, which are then squared to make the shape on the graph. The result is that the difference in size between the squares or rectangles area will be much larger than the difference between the two numbers they represent. Comparing the areas of circles is prone to more error again, for the same reason. It is possible to offset these problems somewhat by choosing a more sophisticated method for encoding the data as an area. Instead of letting the data value be the length of the side of a square or the radius of the circle, for example, we could map the value directly to area and back-calculate the side length or radius. Still, the result will generally not be as good as alternatives. These problems are further compounded for “three-dimensional” shapes like blocks, cylinders, or spheres, which appear to represent volumes. And as saw with the 3-D bar chart in figure 1.10, the perspective or implied viewing angle that accompanies these kinds of charts creates other problems when it comes to reading the scale on a y-axis. Finally, we find it hard to judge changes in slope. The estimation of rates of change in lines or trends is strongly conditioned by the aspect ratio of the graph, as we saw in figure 1.12. Our relatively weak judgment of slopes also interacts badly with threedimensional representations of data. Our ability to scan the “away” dimension of depth (along the z-axis) is weaker than our ability to scan the x- and y-axes. For this reason, it can be disproportionately difficult to interpret data displays of point clouds or surfaces displayed with three axes. They can look impressive, but they are also harder to grasp. 1.5 Channels for Representing Data Graphical elements represent our data in ways that we can see. Different sorts of variables attributes can be represented more or less well by different kinds of visual marks or representations, such as points, lines, shapes, and colors. Our task is to come up with methods that encode or map variables in the right way. As we do this, we face several constraints. First, the channel or mapping that Look at Data we choose needs to be capable of representing the kind of data that we have. If we want to pick out unordered categories, for example, choosing a continuous gradient to represent them will not make much sense. If our variable is continuous, it will not be helpful to represent it as a series of shapes. Second, given that the data can be comprehensibly represented by the visual element we choose, we will want to know how effective that representation is. This was the goal of Cleveland’s research. Following Tamara Munzer (2014, 101–3), Figures 1.24 and 1.25 present an approximate ranking of the effectiveness of different channels for ordered and unordered data, respectively. If we have ordered data and we want the viewer to efficiently make comparisons, then we should try to encode it as a position on a common scale. Encoding numbers as lengths (absent a scale) works too, but not as effectively. Encoding them as areas will make comparisons less accurate again, and so on. Third, the effectiveness of our graphics will depend not just on the channel that we choose but on the perceptual details of how we implement it. So, if we have a measure with four categories ordered from lowest to highest, we might correctly decide to represent it using a sequence of colors. But if we pick the wrong sequence, the data will still be hard to interpret, or actively misleading. In a similar way, if we pick a bad set of hues for an unordered categorical variable, the result might not just be unpleasant to look at but actively misleading. Finally, bear in mind that these different channels or mappings for data are not in themselves kinds of graphs. They are just the elements or building blocks for graphs. When we choose to encode a variable as a position, a length, an area, a shade of gray, or a color, we have made an important decision that narrows down what the resulting plot can look like. But this is not the same as deciding what type of plot it will be, in the sense of choosing whether to make a dotplot or a bar chart, a histogram or a frequency polygon, and so on. 1.6 Problems of Honesty and Good Judgment Figure 1.26 shows two ways of redrawing our life expectancy figure (fig. 1.4). Each of these plots is far less noisy than the junk-filled monstrosity we began with. But they also have design features that 0 5 0 0 10 5 27 Position on a common scale 10 5 • 10 Position on unaligned scales Length Tilt or Angle Area (2D as size) Depth (3D as position) Color Iuminance or brightness Color saturation or intensity Curvature Volume (3D as size) Figure 1.24: Channels for mapping ordered data (continuous or other quantitative measures), arranged top to bottom from more to less effective, after Munzer (2014, 102). 28 • Chapter 1 Position in space Color hue Motion Shape Figure 1.25: Channels for mapping unordered categorical data, arranged top-to-bottom from more to less effective, after Munzer (2014, 102). Oceania Europe Americas Asia Africa 0 20 40 60 80 Life expectancy in years, 2007 Oceania Europe Americas Asia Africa 60 70 80 Life expectancy in years, 2007 Figure 1.26: Two simpler versions of our junk chart. The scale on the bar chart version goes to zero, while the scale on the dotplot version is confined to the range of values taken by the observations. could be argued over and might even matter substantively depending on the circumstances. For example, consider the scales on the x-axis in each case. The left-hand panel in figure 1.26 is a bar chart, and the length of the bar represents the value of the variable “average life expectancy in 2007” for each continent. The scale starts at zero and extends to just beyond the level of the largest value. Meanwhile the right-hand panel is a Cleveland dotplot. Each observation is represented by a point, and the scale is restricted to the range of the data as shown. It is tempting to lay down inflexible rules about what to do in terms of producing your graphs, and to dismiss people who don’t follow them as producing junk charts or lying with statistics. But being honest with your data is a bigger problem than can be solved by rules of thumb about making graphs. In this case there is a moderate level of agreement that bar charts should generally include a zero baseline (or equivalent) given that bars make lengths salient to the viewer. But it would be a mistake to think that a dotplot was by the same token deliberately misleading, just because it kept itself to the range of the data instead. Which one is to be preferred? It is tricky to give an unequivocal answer, because the reasons for preferring one type of scaling over another depend in part on how often people actively try to mislead others by preferring one sort of representation over another. On the one hand, there is a lot of be said in favor of showing the data over the range we observe it, rather than forcing every scale to encompass its lowest and highest theoretical value. Many otherwise informative visualizations would become useless if it was mandatory to include a zero point on the x- or y-axis. On the other hand, it’s also true that people sometimes go out of their way to restrict the scales they display in a way that makes their argument look better. Sometimes this is done out of active malice, other times out of passive bias, or even just a hopeful desire to see what you want to see in the data. (Remember, often the main audience for your visualizations is yourself.) In those cases, the resulting graphic will indeed be misleading. Rushed, garish, and deliberately inflammatory or misleading graphics are a staple of social media sharing and the cable news cycle. But the problem comes up in everyday practice as well, and the two can intersect if your work ends up in front of a public audience. For example, let’s take a look at some historical data on law school enrollments. A decline in enrollments led to some 1.7 Think Clearly about Graphs I am going to assume that your goal is to draw effective graphs in an honest and reproducible way. Default settings and general rules of good practice have limited powers to stop you from doing the wrong thing. But one thing they can do is provide not just tools for making graphs but also a framework or set of concepts that helps you think more clearly about the good work you want to produce. When learning a graphing system or toolkit, people • 29 50,000 45,000 40,000 1980 1990 2000 2010 2000 2010 Year First year enrollment reporting on trends since the early 1970s. The results are shown in figure 1.27. The first panel shows the trend in the number of students beginning law school each year since 1973. The y-axis starts from just below the lowest value in the series. The second panel shows the same data but with the y-axis minimum set to zero instead. The columnist and writer Justin Fox saw the first version and remarked on how amazing it was. He was then quite surprised at the strong reactions he got from people who insisted the y-axis should have included zero. The original chart was “possibly . . . one of the worst represented charts I’ve ever seen,” said one interlocutor. Another remarked that “graphs that don’t go to zero are a thought crime” (Fox 2014). My own view is that the chart without the zero baseline shows you that, after almost forty years of mostly rising enrollments, law school enrollments dropped suddenly and precipitously around 2011 to levels not seen since the early 1970s. The levels are clearly labeled, and the decline does look substantively surprising and significant. In a well-constructed chart the axis labels are a necessary guide to the reader, and we should expect readers to pay attention to them. The chart with the zero baseline, meanwhile, does not add much additional information beyond reminding you, at the cost of wasting some space, that 35,000 is a number quite a lot larger than zero. That said, I am sympathetic to people who got upset at the first chart. At a minimum, it shows they know to read the axis labels on a graph. That is less common than you might think. It likely also shows they know interfering with the axes is one way to make a chart misleading, and that it is not unusual for that sort of thing to be done deliberately. First year enrollment Look at Data 40,000 20,000 0 1980 1990 Year Figure 1.27: Two views of the rapid decline in law school enrollments in the mid-2010s. 30 • Chapter 1 often start thinking about specific ways they want their graph to look. They quickly start formulating requests. They want to know how to make a particular kind of chart, or how to change the typeface for the whole graph, or how to adjust the scales, or how to move the title, customize the labels, or change the colors of the points. These requests involve different features of the graph. Some have to do with basic features of the figure’s structure, with which bits of data are encoded as or mapped to elements such as shape, line, or color. Some have to do with the details of how those elements are represented. If a variable is mapped to shape, which shapes will be chosen, exactly? If another variable is represented by color, which colors in particular will be used? Some have to do with the framing or guiding features of the graph. If there are tickmarks on the x-axis, can I decide where they should be drawn? If the chart has a legend, will it appear to the right of the graph or on top? If data points have information encoded in both shape and color, do we need a separate legend for each encoding, or can we combine them into a single unified legend? And some have to do with thematic features of the graph that may greatly affect how the final result looks but are not logically connected to the structure of the data being represented. Can I change the title font from Times New Roman to Helvetica? Can I have a light blue background in all my graphs? A real strength of ggplot is that it implements a grammar of graphics to organize and make sense of these different elements (Wilkinson 2005). Instead of a huge, conceptually flat list of options for setting every aspect of a plot’s appearance at once, ggplot breaks up the task of making a graph into a series of distinct tasks, each bearing a well-defined relationship to the structure of the plot. When you write your code, you carry out each task using a function that controls that part of the job. At the beginning, ggplot will do most of the work for you. Only two steps are required. First, you must give some information to the ggplot() function. This establishes the core of the plot by saying what data you are using and what variables will be linked or mapped to features of the plot. Second, you must choose a geom_ function. This decides what sort of plot will be drawn, such as a scatterplot, a bar chart, or a boxplot. As you progress, you will gradually use other functions to gain more fine-grained control over other features of the plot, such as scales, legends, and thematic elements. This also means that, as Look at Data you learn ggplot, it is very important to grasp the core steps first, before worrying about adjustments and polishing. And so that is how we’ll proceed. In the next chapter we will learn how to get up and running in R and make our first graphs. From there, we will work through examples that introduce each element of ggplot’s way of doing things. We will be producing sophisticated plots quite quickly, and we will keep working on them until we are in full control of what we are doing. As we go, we will learn about some ideas and associated techniques and tricks to make R do what we want. 1.8 Where to Go Next For an entertaining and informative overview of various visual effects and optical “illusions,” take a look at Michael Bach’s website at michaelbach.de. If you would like to learn more about the relationship between perception and data visualization, follow up on some of the references in this chapter. Munzer (2014), Ware (2008), and Few (2009) are good places to start. William Cleveland’s books (1993, 1994) are models of clarity and good advice. As we shall see beginning in the next chapter, the ideas developed in Wilkinson (2005) are at the heart of ggplot’s approach to visualization. Finally, foundational work by Bertin (2010) lies behind a lot of thinking on the relationship between data and visual elements. • 31 2 Get Started In this chapter, we will begin to learn how to create pictures of data that people, including ourselves, can look at and learn from. R and ggplot are the tools we will use. The best way to learn them is to follow along and repeatedly write code as you go. The material in this book is designed to be interactive and hands-on. If you work through it with me using the approach described below, you will end up with a book much like this one, with many code samples alongside your notes, and the figures or other output produced by that code shown nearby. I strongly encourage you to type out your code rather than copying and pasting the examples from the text. Typing it out will help you learn it. At the beginning it may feel like tedious transcription you don’t fully understand. But it slows you down in a way that gets you used to what the syntax and structure of R are like and is a very effective way to learn the language. It’s especially useful for ggplot, where the code for our figures will repeatedly have a similar structure, built up piece by piece. 2.1 Work in Plain Text, Using RMarkdown When taking notes, and when writing your own code, you should write plain text in a text editor. Do not use Microsoft Word or some other word processor. You may be used to thinking of your final outputs (e.g., a Word file, a PDF document, presentation slides, or the tables and figures you make) as what’s “real” about your project. Instead, it’s better to think of the data and code as what’s real, together with the text you write. The idea is that all your finished output—your figures, tables, text, and so on—can be procedurally and reproducibly generated from code, data, and written material stored in a simple, plain-text format. The ability to reproduce your work in this way is important to the scientific process. But you should also see it as a pragmatic choice that will make life easier for you in the future. The reality Get Started for most of us is that the person who will most want to easily reproduce your work is you, six months or a year from now. This is especially true for graphics and figures. These often have a “finished” quality to them, as a result of much tweaking and adjustments to the details of the figure. That can make them hard to reproduce later. While it is normal for graphics to undergo a substantial amount of polishing on their way to publication, our goal is to do as much of this as possible programmatically, in code we write, rather than in a way that is retrospectively invisible, as, for example, when we edit an image in an application like Adobe Illustrator. While learning ggplot, and later while doing data analysis, you will find yourself constantly pinging back and forth between three things: 1. 2. 3. Writing code. You will write a lot of code to produce plots. You will also write code to load your data and to look quickly at tables of that data. Sometimes you will want to summarize, rearrange, subset, or augment your data, or run a statistical model with it. You will want to be able to write that code as easily and effectively as possible. Looking at output. Your code is a set of instructions that, when executed, produces the output you want: a table, a model, or a figure. It is often helpful to be able to see that output and its partial results. While we’re working, it’s also useful to keep the code and the things produced by the code close together, if we can. Taking notes. You will also be writing about what we are doing and what your results mean. When learning how to do something in ggplot, for instance, you will want to make notes to yourself about what you did, why you wrote it this way rather than that, or what this new concept, function, or instruction does. Later, when doing data analysis and making figures, you will be writing up reports or drafting papers. How can you do all this effectively? The simplest way to keep code and notes together is to write your code and intersperse it with comments. All programming languages have some way of demarcating lines as comments, usually by putting a special character (like #) at the start of the line. We could create a plain-text script file called, e.g., notes.r, containing code and our comments on it. This is fine as far as it goes. But except for very short files, it • 33 34 • Chapter 2 Report notes.pdf We can see this relationship in a scatterplot. will be difficult to do anything useful with the comments we write. If we want a report from an analysis, for example, we will have to write it up separately. While a script file can keep comments and code together, it loses the connection between code and its output, such as the figure we want to produce. But there is a better alternative: we can write our notes using RMarkdown. An RMarkdown file is just a plain text document where text (such as notes or discussion) is interspersed with pieces, or chunks, of R code. When you feed the document to R, it knits this file into a new document by running the R code piece by piece, in sequence, and either supplementing or replacing the chunks of code with their output. The resulting file is then converted into a more readable document formatted in HTML, PDF, or Word. The noncode segments of the document are plain text, but they can have simple formatting instructions in them. These are set using Markdown, a set of conventions for marking up plain text in a way that indicates how it should be formatted. The basic elements of Markdown are shown in the upper part of figure 2.1. When you create a markdown document in R Studio, it contains some sample text to get you started. RMarkdown documents look like the one shown schematically in the lower part of figure 2.1. Your notes or text, with Markdown formatting as needed, are interspersed with code. There is a set format for code chunks. They look like this: ```{r} y ``` x As you can see, this plot looks pretty nice. Figure 2.1: Top: Some elements of RMarkdown syntax. Bottom: From a plain text RMarkdown file to PDF output. The format is language agnostic and can be used with, e.g., Python and other languages. Three backticks (on a U.S. keyboard, that’s the character under the escape key) are followed by a pair of curly braces containing the name of the language we are using. The backticks-and-braces part signals that a chunk of code is about to begin. You write your code as needed and then end the chunk with a new line containing just three backticks. If you keep your notes in this way, you will be able to see the code you wrote, the output it produces, and your own commentary or clarification on it in a convenient way. Moreover, you can turn it into a good-looking document right away. Get Started • 35 Figure 2.2: Bare-bones R running from the Terminal. 2.2 Use R with RStudio The RStudio environment R itself is a relatively small application with next to no user interface. Everything works through a command line, or console. At its most basic, you launch it from your Terminal application (on a Mac) or Command Prompt (on Windows) by typing R. Once launched, R awaits your instructions at a command line of its own, denoted by the right angle bracket symbol, > (fig. 2.2). When you
type an instruction and hit return, R interprets it and sends any
resulting output back to the console.
In addition to interacting with the console, you can also write
your code in a text file and send that to R all at once. You can use
any good text editor to write your .r scripts. But although a plain
text file and a command line are the absolute minimum you need
to work with R, it is a rather spa…

Calculate your order
Pages (275 words)
Standard price: $0.00
Client Reviews
4.9
Sitejabber
4.6
Trustpilot
4.8
Our Guarantees
100% Confidentiality
Information about customers is confidential and never disclosed to third parties.
Original Writing
We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
Timely Delivery
No missed deadlines – 97% of assignments are completed in time.
Money Back
If you're confident that a writer didn't follow your order details, ask for a refund.

Calculate the price of your order

You will get a personal manager and a discount.
We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
Total price:
$0.00
Power up Your Academic Success with the
Team of Professionals. We’ve Got Your Back.
Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.
WeCreativez WhatsApp Support
Our customer support team is here to answer your questions. Ask us anything!
? Hi, how can I help?