PSY 101 KSU Adolescent Friendships & Romantic Relationships Summary

The article contains an Abstract, Introduction, Method, Results, Discussion, and References section2.

  • Your summary of the article
  • Statement of the purpose of the research
  • The specific hypothesis tested
  • Summary of the methods used and procedures followed
  • Summary of the results
  • Summary of the major implications of the research
  • Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology
    2005, Vol. 34, No. 1, 49–61
    Copyright © 2005 by
    Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
    Adolescent Peer Relations, Friendships, and Romantic Relationships:
    Do They Predict Social Anxiety and Depression?
    Annette M. La Greca and Hannah Moore Harrison
    Department of Psychology, University of Miami
    This study examined multiple levels of adolescents’interpersonal functioning, including general peer relations (peer crowd affiliations, peer victimization), and qualities
    of best friendships and romantic relationships as predictors of symptoms of depression and social anxiety. An ethnically diverse sample of 421 adolescents (57% girls;
    14 to 19 years) completed measures of peer crowd affiliation, peer victimization, and
    qualities of best friendships and romantic relationships. Peer crowd affiliations (high
    and low status), positive qualities in best friendships, and the presence of a dating relationship protected adolescents against feelings of social anxiety, whereas relational
    victimization and negative interactions in best friendships predicted high social anxiety. In contrast, affiliation with a high-status peer crowd afforded some protection
    against depressive affect; however, relational victimization and negative qualities of
    best friendships and romantic relationships predicted depressive symptoms. Some
    moderating effects for ethnicity were observed. Findings indicate that multiple aspects of adolescents’ social relations uniquely contribute to feelings of internal distress. Implications for research and preventive interventions are discussed.
    Adolescence is a critical period in social development, marked by an expansion of peer networks, increased importance of close friendships, and the emergence of romantic relationships. As adolescents make
    the transition to middle school and then high school,
    peer networks increase, and peer crowd affiliation becomes an important aspect of peer relations (La Greca
    & Prinstein, 1999). Also during adolescence, close
    friends begin to surpass parents as adolescents’ primary source of social support and contribute in important ways to adolescents’ self-concept and well-being
    (Furman & Buhrmester, 1992). In addition, dating relationships emerge and take on increasing importance.
    By age 16, a majority of adolescents report having had a
    romantic relationship (Carver, Joyner, & Udry, 2003).
    Reflecting these important changes in social relations, a growing number of studies have examined
    linkages between adolescents’ peer relations and internalizing aspects of mental health, such as feelings of
    depression and social anxiety (e.g., Hecht, Inderbitzen,
    & Bukowski, 1998; Inderbitzen, Walters, & Bukowski,
    1997; La Greca & Lopez, 1998; Vernberg, 1990; Vernberg, Abwender, Ewell, & Beery, 1992). Symptoms of
    depression and social anxiety are particularly impor-
    tant to study during adolescence, as both are common
    (Birmaher et al., 1996; La Greca & Lopez, 1998;
    Petersen et al., 1993) and may be risk factors for impairment in adulthood (Aalto-Setaelae, Marttunen,
    Tuulio-Henriksson, Poikolainen, & Loennqvist, 2002;
    Devine, Kempton, & Forehand, 1994). Symptoms of
    depression and social anxiety may also be precursors
    to more severe psychopathology, including major depressive disorder and social anxiety disorder, which
    are chronic disorders that often originate in adolescence and continue into adulthood (Birmaher et al.,
    1996; Moutier & Stein, 1999).
    In view of such findings, it is especially critical to
    identify social risk and protective factors for depression and social anxiety among adolescents. A better
    understanding of risk and protective factors can contribute to theory development and to the development
    of preventive interventions for internalizing disorders
    (Lewinsohn et al., 1994). Thus, this study builds on and
    extends prior research by examining aspects of adolescents’ general peer relations, best friendships, and romantic relationships, in an effort to comprehensively
    examine the role of peer and social relations in the development of adolescents’ feelings of depression and
    social anxiety. Both positive and negative aspects of
    general peer relations, best friendships, and romantic
    relationships were considered, as described in the following. Separate models of risk and protective factors
    were examined for depression and social anxiety, as
    these are related but distinct aspects of internal distress
    (Inderbitzen et al., 1997; Inderbitzen-Nolan & Walters,
    The authors thank the teachers and principals at the participating
    schools and express appreciation to the research assistants who contributed their time and efforts to this project, especially Ami F.
    Kuttler and Alissa R. Glickman.
    Requests for reprints should be sent to Annette M. LaGreca, P.O.
    Box 249229, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL 33124. E-mail:
    alagreca@miami.edu
    49
    LA GRECA AND HARRISON
    The first and broadest level of social functioning examined in this study focused on two different aspects of
    adolescents’ general peer relations: peer crowd affiliations and peer victimization. Although “peer crowds”
    and “peer victimization” are distinct constructs, both
    may be conceptualized as reflecting aspects of adolescents’ social relations that occur within the larger peer
    system (i.e., the large set of adolescents with whom an
    individual regularly interacts).
    who keep to themselves (Loners or Nobodies). Although the specific terms for these crowds may vary,
    the peer crowds described here are remarkably similar
    across gender, regions, and ethnic groups (e.g., Brown,
    1990; Dolcini & Adler, 1994; Phillips, Hughes, &
    Wilkes, 1998).
    Adolescents cite social support, friendship development, and the facilitation of social interactions as key
    benefits of peer crowd affiliation (Brown, Eicher, &
    Petrie, 1986). Moreover, because peer crowd affiliation
    may be “reputation based” (i.e., adolescents are often
    labeled by their peers based on perceived characteristics), crowd affiliations also reflect adolescents’ acceptance, as well as their power, within the larger peer system (Brown, 1990). Jocks and Populars represent
    high-status crowds, whereas Burnouts and Alternatives typically reflect low-status crowds (La Greca,
    Prinstein, & Fetter, 2001; Prinstein & La Greca, 1998).
    Although peer crowds are distinct from adolescents’
    smaller peer networks (or cliques) and close friendships (La Greca & Prinstein, 1999), most adolescents
    have one or more close friends who affiliate with the
    same peer crowd (La Greca et al., 2001; Urberg et al.,
    1995).
    Little research has examined linkages between peer
    crowd affiliations and adolescents’ internal distress.
    Some evidence suggests that adolescents who affiliate
    with high-status groups (Jocks, Populars) report higher
    self-esteem and less loneliness than other adolescents,
    whereas adolescents who affiliate with a low-status
    crowd (Burnouts, Alternatives) report more depressed
    affect and lower self-esteem than others (Brown &
    Lohr, 1987; Prinstein & La Greca, 2002). Linkages between peer crowd affiliations and social anxiety are
    less clear, although affiliations with high-status crowds
    may be associated with less social anxiety (Prinstein &
    La Greca, 2002). This study examined whether affiliation with high-status crowds represents a protective
    factor, and whether affiliation with low-status crowds
    represents a risk factor for internal distress.
    Adolescent Peer Crowds
    Peer Victimization
    Acceptance by peers is an important part of adolescent self-identity and has a strong influence on psychological adjustment (Harter, 1997). In part to seek support and acceptance from peers, many adolescents
    affiliate with a peer crowd. Peer crowds are “reputation-based collectives of similarly stereotyped individuals who may or may not spend much time together”
    (Brown, 1990, p. 177). Specifically, peer crowds include a high-status, image-oriented crowd (Populars or
    Hot Shots); an athletically oriented crowd (Jocks or
    Athletes); an academically oriented crowd (Brains); a
    crowd that rebels against social norms (Alternatives
    or Nonconformists); a deviant, rule-breaking crowd
    (Burnouts, Dirts, or Druggies); and a crowd of misfits
    Peer victimization and aversive experiences with
    peers (e.g., exclusion, aggression) represents a line of
    research that focuses on negative aspects of the larger
    peer system. Studies with children and adolescents
    have consistently linked peer victimization with internal distress, including feelings of depression and loneliness (e.g., Crick & Bigbee, 1998; Prinstein, Boergers,
    & Vernberg, 2001; Vernberg, 1990).
    Although initial research focused on overt peer victimization, such as physical violence and threats, finding that boys are most often the aggressors and the victims (e.g., Nansel et al., 2001), recent research has
    expanded the focus to include relational victimization,
    such as rumor spreading, friendship withdrawal, and
    2000; La Greca & Lopez, 1998) and may have different
    patterns of associations with social functioning.
    Furthermore, potential gender differences in depression and social anxiety were considered, as adolescent girls typically report more symptoms of depression and social anxiety than adolescent boys (e.g.,
    Inderbitzen-Nolan & Walters, 2000; La Greca &
    Lopez, 1998). We also explored the possibility that the
    linkages between adolescents’ social functioning and
    reports of depression and social anxiety might differ by
    gender. Adolescent girls typically have more close
    friends than boys and report more intimacy in their
    friendships (Berndt, 1982; Urberg, Degirmencioglu,
    Tolson, & Halliday-Scher, 1995); thus, problems in
    close friendships or romantic relationships might lead
    to greater internal distress for girls than for boys. Some
    research suggests that problems in close friendships
    (i.e., less intimacy and companionship) are more
    strongly related to social anxiety for adolescent girls
    than for boys (e.g., La Greca & Lopez, 1998; Vernberg
    et al., 1992). At the same time, however, problems in
    adolescents’ general peer relations (e.g., peer rejection) have been associated with internal distress for
    both boys and girls (Interbitzen et al., 1997). This study
    afforded the opportunity to explore gender differences
    in the patterns of associations between adolescents’ social functioning and their internal distress.
    General Peer Relations
    50
    PEER RELATIONS
    social exclusion (Crick & Grotpeter, 1996; Prinstein et
    al., 2001; Vernberg, Jacobs, & Hershberger, 1999).
    Among adolescents, boys and girls report that relational victimization is more common than overt victimization (Prinstein et al., 2001), and although boys
    report more overt victimization than girls, gender differences in relational victimization are less clear (Prinstein et al., 2001; Vernberg et al., 1999).
    Studies with early adolescents have found that
    aversive peer experiences and peer victimization lead
    to increases in depression and social anxiety over time
    (Vernberg, 1990; Vernberg et al., 1992). However, research with older adolescents has been very limited. In
    one recent study, both overt and relational victimization were related to adolescents’ reports of internal distress (i.e., depression, loneliness, and low self-esteem;
    Prinstein et al., 2001).
    This study examined both relational and overt victimization. Relational victimization was expected to
    predict both depression and social anxiety. Moreover,
    overt victimization was expected to add to the prediction of depressive symptoms, as in previous work
    (Prinstein et al., 2001). However, it was less clear how
    overt victimization would relate to social anxiety, as
    adolescents who are the recipients of overt victimization may also be physically aggressive (Prinstein et al.,
    2001), rather than being shy, avoidant, or anxious
    around peers.
    Best Friendships
    The second level of adolescent social functioning
    examined in this study focused on adolescents’ close
    friendships. Close friendships are essential to the development of interpersonal intimacy, empathy, and
    perspective-taking skills, and a high-quality friendship
    might lessen the harmful effects of low peer acceptance (Buhrmester, 1990; Sullivan, 1953). During adolescence, friendships are a primary source of social
    support (Furman & Buhrmester, 1992). In general, the
    positive qualities of adolescents’ close friendships (i.e.,
    intimacy, support) have been associated with lower
    levels of social anxiety (La Greca & Lopez, 1998;
    Vernberg et al., 1992), suggesting that close friendships may serve a protective mental health function. In
    fact, support from close friends has been associated
    with positive self-esteem and better psychosocial adjustment (e.g., Buhrmester, 1990; Compas, Slavin,
    Wagner, & Vannatta, 1986).
    Despite their benefits, close friendships may also
    have negative features, such as conflict, pressure, and
    exclusion (Furman & Buhrmester, 1985). Negative aspects of adolescents’ friendships have been linked to
    problems with self-esteem and school adjustment
    (Berndt & Keefe, 1995). Although it is likely that negative interactions with a best friend may contribute to
    depressive symptoms, this has not been directly ex-
    amined. However, adolescents’ feelings of depression
    have been associated with less social participation and
    more interpersonal problems (e.g., Mufson, Weissman,
    Moreau, & Garfinkel, 1999). Similarly, negative interactions within best friendships may contribute to feelings of social anxiety, as conflict with or exclusion
    from a best friend may heighten feelings of discomfort
    or distress around peers and raise concerns about negative peer evaluations. Thus, this study examined linkages between the positive and negative features of adolescents’ best friendships and their internal distress.
    Romantic Relationships
    Romantic relationships represent the third level of
    adolescent social relations examined in this study. Romantic relationships emerge for the first time in adolescence. Dating typically begins around 14 to 15 years of
    age, initially as an extension of involvement in mixedgender peer groups (Connolly, Craig, Goldberg, &
    Pepler, 1999; Feiring, 1996). An accumulating body
    of research has dispelled the notion that adolescent dating relationships are transitory and unimportant (Collins, 2003; Davila, Steinberg, Kachadourian, Cobb, &
    Fincham, 2004).
    Adolescent romantic relationships are similar to
    close friendships in that both involve support, intimacy, and companionship (Laursen, 1996). Distinctions also exist, as adolescents name passion, commitment, and sexual intimacy as characteristics specific
    only to romantic relationships (Connolly et al., 1999).
    By late adolescence, adolescents report greater closeness with romantic partners than with best friends, parents, and siblings (Laursen, 1996).
    Although little research has examined this issue, a
    positive, romantic relationship during adolescence
    might provide an important source of support and contribute in positive ways to adolescents’ mental health.
    In contrast, negative experiences could contribute to
    internal distress. Recent work suggests that adolescents who date, particularly those who experience
    stress in their romantic relationships, report higher levels of depressive symptoms than their nondating peers
    (Davila et al., 2004). Thus, it is likely that negative interactions with a romantic partner would be distressing
    to adolescents and elicit feelings of depression or social
    anxiety. Moreover, depressed adolescents may choose
    less positive romantic partners (Daley & Hammen,
    2002).
    Despite the importance of adolescent romantic relationships, few studies have examined the linkage between relationship quality and adjustment. Moreover,
    existing studies have not examined whether romantic
    relationships make independent contributions to the
    prediction of adjustment beyond the impact of close
    friendships or general peer relations. This is an important consideration, because adolescents who have
    51
    LA GRECA AND HARRISON
    better quality romantic relationships also have better
    quality friendships and higher social competence
    (Connolly, Furman, & Konarski, 2000; Kuttler & La
    Greca, 2004).
    Summary of This Study
    This study examined multiple levels of interpersonal functioning as potential risk and protective factors for symptoms of depression and social anxiety.
    Specifically, the study focused on general peer relations (peer crowd affiliation, peer victimization), best
    friendships, and romantic relationships and incorporated positive and negative features of these social relations. An ethnically diverse sample of adolescents participated, as the study was conducted in South Florida,
    which has a large Hispanic population. In prior work,
    Hispanic adolescents have not differed from non-Hispanic White adolescents in terms of their social anxiety
    (La Greca, 1999) or dating status (Kuttler & La Greca,
    2004). In addition, adolescent dating involvement has
    been found to be remarkably similar across most ethnic
    groups (see Collins, 2003). Although ethnicity was not
    a main focus of the study, exploratory analyses evaluated whether similar patterns of social functioning and
    internal distress were apparent for Hispanic and nonHispanic youth.
    Method
    Participants
    The participants were 421 adolescents (247 girls;
    57%) enrolled in a public high school in a large metropolitan area in the Southeast. Adolescents ranged from
    14 to 19 years of age (M = 16.5, SD = 1.0); 30% were in
    the 10th grade, 32% were in the 11th grade, and 38%
    were in the 12th grade. The adolescents came from primarily middle-class socioeconomic backgrounds as reflected in the Hollingshead Index (father’s occupation
    Mdn = 7.00, SD = 2.31; mother’s occupation: Mdn =
    6.00, SD = 1.99, where 1 = occupations that require no
    training and 9 = occupations that require graduate
    school). The ethnic composition of the sample, similar
    to the surrounding county, was 67.1% Hispanic, 17.4%
    non-Hispanic White, 9.0% Black, and 6.4% mixed/
    other. Among the Hispanic youth who reported their
    family’s country of origin, the most frequently mentioned were Cuba, Nicaragua, and Columbia.
    Procedure
    Adolescents were recruited as part of a larger study
    of adolescent health and peer relations. Homeroom
    teachers distributed consent forms to adolescents, who
    brought them home for parents’ consideration. Active
    52
    parental consent was obtained for adolescents under
    the age of 18 years. Consent forms were returned for
    approximately 50% of the adolescents, and 98% indicated permission for the adolescent to participate. Adolescent assent also was obtained.
    The study measures included the Peer Crowd Questionnaire, the Revised Peer Experiences Questionnaire, the Network of Relationships–Revised, the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents, and the revised
    Beck Depression Inventory; the order of administration was always the same, but other nonstudy measures
    were interspersed throughout the protocol. Adolescents also provided demographic information. The
    study took place in the early spring (March and April).
    Trained graduate and undergraduate assistants administered questionnaires during group testing sessions
    that lasted approximately 45 min. All information was
    provided anonymously.
    Measures
    Peer Crowd Questionnaire (Brown, 1990; La
    Greca et al., 2001). The Peer Crowd Questionnaire
    assessed adolescents’ social status via their primary affiliation with a particular peer crowd. Based on prior
    research (Brown, 1990; Mosbach & Leventhal, 1988),
    several common crowds were listed: Jocks (athletic, on
    school team), Burnouts (skip school, get into trouble),
    Brains (do well in school, enjoy academics), Populars
    (involved in activities, concerned about image), Alternatives (rebel against the norm in clothing or ideas, do
    not conform to social ideals), and None/Average (no
    affiliation, or “just average”). For each crowd, adolescents indicated (yes or no) whether the crowd was present in their school, listed any alternate names for the
    crowds, and described any additional crowds that existed in their schools. (Adolescents identified all the
    key peer crowds listed.) Next, adolescents selected the
    crowd with which they most identified.
    In an independent sample (N = 323), we found
    strong associations between adolescents’ primary peer
    crowd affiliation and their ratings of the degree to
    which they identified with that peer crowd (on a
    5-point scale). Specifically, using analyses of variance,
    Jocks rated their identification with the Jock crowd (M
    = 4.2) significantly higher than all other adolescents,
    Burnouts rated their identification with the Burnout
    crowd (M = 4.5) significantly higher than all other adolescents, and so on for the other peer crowds (all ps < .0001). Other studies have shown good correspondence between adolescents’ self-identification and peers’ assignment to crowds (Brown et al., 1986). In addition, based on adolescents’ self-reports, Sussman et al. (1990) had two independent raters assign adolescents to peer crowds from choices of Freaks (Burnouts), Jocks, Hotshots/Populars, and Regulars/Average, yielding an interrater reliability of 93%. PEER RELATIONS Based on prior research (e.g., La Greca et al., 2001; Prinstein et al., 2001), the Populars and Jocks were considered to be high-status peer crowds, the Burnouts and Alternatives were low-status, and all others were average. In regression analyses, dummy coding (1 = presence; 0 = absence) was used to identify adolescents who affiliated with the high-status or low-status crowds. Revised Peer Experiences Questionnaire (Prinstein et al., 2001). The Revised Peer Experiences Questionnaire was developed to assess peer aggression and victimization in adolescents; in this study only the victimization subscale was used. The victimization items reflect aversive peer experiences and are rated on frequency of occurrence ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (a few times a week). Five items reflect relational victimization (“A teen left me out of what he or she was doing”), and four reflect overt victimization (“A teen kicked, hit, or punched me in a mean way”). Scores for relational and overt victimization were obtained by summing the relevant items. Prinstein et al. (2001) reported excellent psychometric support for the victimization scales and significant relations between overt and relational aggression and adolescents’ reports of internal distress. In this sample, internal consistencies were .82 (relational victimization) and .78 (overt victimization). Network of Relationship Inventory–Revised (Furman & Buhrmester, 1985). The Network of Relationship Inventory–Revised assessed the qualities of adolescents’ best friendships and romantic relationships. The inventory measures nine positive qualities (i.e., companionship, affection, disclosure, nurturance, instrumental aid, approval, support, reliable alliance, and satisfaction) and five negative qualities (i.e., conflict, criticism, exclusion, dominance, and pressure). Each factor is assessed by three items that are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (little or none) to 5 (the most). Adolescents completed the scales for their best friendship (same-sex) and their romantic partner (if applicable). A romantic partner was defined as “someone you are physically attracted to, have had intimate contact with (e.g., hand holding, kissing, etc.), you consider to be more than a friend, and go out on ‘dates’ with.” Summary scores were calculated (separately for best friends and romantic partners) for positive and negative relationship qualities by averaging all relevant items. Internal consistencies were .94 to .95 (positive qualities) and .83 to .84 (negative qualities). To include all adolescents in the regression analyses, those who did not have a romantic partner were assigned scores of 1 (none) for positive and negative qualities of their romantic relationships. Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (La Greca & Lopez, 1998). The Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents assessed adolescents’ anxiety in social situations with peers. It contains 18 items rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (all the time). Total scores are obtained by summing relevant items and can range from 18 to 90. High socially anxious adolescents have reported more problems in their peer relations than low socially anxious teens (Inderbitzen et al., 1997; La Greca & Lopez, 1998; Vernberg et al., 1992). In this study, the internal consistency of the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents Total was .87. Revised Beck Depression Inventory (Beck & Steer, 1987). This 21-item scale assesses severity of cognitive, affective, and somatic symptoms of depression. Items are rated from 0 to 3, based on severity. Totals can range from 0 to 63. The BDI has demonstrated good reliability and validity, as well as usefulness with adolescents (Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). In this study, scores ranged from 0 to 52, and internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) was .91. Results Preliminary Analyses Descriptive Information Prior to the main analyses, descriptive information was obtained for the main study variables. First, 31.2% of the adolescents reported affiliating with a high-status peer crowd (Jocks or Populars), and this did not differ by gender. In addition, 14.8% of the adolescents reported affiliating with one of the low-status peer crowds (Burnouts or Alternatives), with more boys reporting such an affiliation (19.7%) than girls (11.3%), χ2 = 5.47, p < .05. These results are consistent with prior research on peer crowd affiliations (e.g., La Greca et al., 2001; Mosbach & Leventhal, 1988; Urberg et al., 1995). For romantic relationships, significantly more girls (64.0%) than boys (41.4%) reported having a romantic partner, χ2 = 20.51, p < .001; these findings are consistent with reports in other studies (e.g., Carver et al., 2003; Glickman & La Greca, 2004). Table 1 lists the means for the variables by gender. No gender differences were observed for social anxiety and depressive symptoms. However, boys reported more relational and overt victimization than girls. For relationship qualities, girls reported more positive interactions with best friends than did boys, but there were no gender differences for positive interactions with romantic partners. In contrast, boys reported more negative interactions in their best friendships and romantic relationships than did girls. Similar analyses were conducted using ethnicity as a grouping variable; however, no significant ethnic differences were obtained. Finally, age was not related to any of the study 53 LA GRECA AND HARRISON Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations of Variables by Gender Girlsa Totalc M SD M SD M SD F Value (1, 419) 7.81 5.09 3.2 2.1 8.61 5.95 3.3 2.4 8.14 5.44 3.3 2.3 6.11** 14.75*** 4.14 1.79 4.19 1.94 38.26 12.39 .65 .54 .73 .56 11.3 9.8 3.69 2.02 4.15 2.13 38.24 10.84 .70 .58 .63 .73 12.6 10.2 3.95 1.88 4.17 2.00 38.26 11.75 .71 .57 .70 .62 11.9 10.0 43.85*** 17.53*** 0.12 4.49* 0.0 2.46 Measure Peer Experiences Relational Victimization Overt Victimization Network of Relationships Friend—Positive Qualities Friend—Negative Qualities Partner—Positive Qualitiesd Partner—Negative Qualitiesd Social Anxiety Scale—total Beck Depression—total Boysb Note: These means were calculated only for the adolescents who reported having a romantic partner. For girls, n = 158; for boys, n = 72. an = 247. bn = 174. cn = 421. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Table 2. Correlations Among Study Variables 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. High-status peer crowd Low-status peer crowd Relational victimization Overt victimization Positive—friend Negative—friend Positive—romantica Negative—romantica Social anxiety Depressive symptoms 2 3 4 5 6 7a 8a 9 10 –.28** 1.00 –.01 .00 1.00 –.02 .08 .52** 1.00 .12 –.08 –.08 –.05 1.00 .02 .07 .24** .33** –.24** 1.00 .01 .02 –.09 –.06 .20* –.25** 1.00 .00 .04 .21** .25** –.10 .51** –.13 1.00 –.14* –.08 .43** .17** –.18** .21** –.19* .15 1.00 –.14* .04 .34** .25** –.04 .25** –.14 .38** .39** 1.00 aOnly adolescents who reported having a romantic relationship are included; n = 230. *p < .01. **p < .001. variables, including dating status (rs ranged from –.06 to .08, ps = ns). Correlations. To aid in the regression analyses, correlations among the predictor and outcome variables were examined (see Table 2; only correlations significant at p < .01 are marked). Adolescents who affiliated with one of the high-status peer crowds (Populars, Jocks) reported less social anxiety and depression than other adolescents, although affiliation with low-status crowds (Burnouts, Alternatives) was not related to social anxiety or depression. For peer victimization, both relational and overt victimization were associated with higher levels of depression and social anxiety. For friendships, the positive and negative qualities of best friendships were related to social anxiety (fewer positive and more negative qualities were associated with higher social anxiety); however, only negative interactions with best friends were related to depression. A similar pattern was observed for the qualities of romantic relationships. In addition, several of the “predictor” variables were interrelated. Specifically, relational and overt victimization were highly correlated (r = .52), as were the 54 negative qualities of best friendships and romantic relationships (r = .51). Relational and overt victimization were moderately correlated with negative interactions in best friendships (rs = .24, .33, respectively) and romantic relationships (rs = .21, .25, respectively). Adolescents who reported more positive qualities in their relationships also reported fewer negative interactions in these same relationships (rs = –.24 for best friends, –.25 for romantic relationships). Hierarchical Regression Models Predicting Social Anxiety and Depressive Symptoms Overview of analyses. Separate hierarchical regression analyses were conducted for social anxiety and depression. In each case, gender was entered on the first step. On Step 2, the variables that reflected the most general level of peer relations were entered, namely peer crowds (high status, low status) and peer victimization (relational, overt). On Step 3, the positive and negative qualities of adolescents’ best friendships were entered. On Step 4, adolescents’ dating status (presence of a dating partner) was entered to account PEER RELATIONS for the fact that not all the adolescents had a dating or romantic partner. Finally, in Step 5, the positive and negative qualities of the romantic relationships were entered. These five steps only tested main effects and are depicted in Tables 3 and 4. As described later, interaction terms were tested in additional steps, to evaluate potential moderation effects for gender and ethnicity; because there were very few findings, only significant interactions are reported in the text. Given the correlations among key predictor variables, this hierarchical regression procedure allowed us to examine the unique contributions of adolescents’ close relationships (best friends, romantic relationships) beyond the effects of general peer relations. In addition, the hierarchical analyses allowed us to examine whether the effects of a predictor variable changed when additional predictors were considered (i.e., controlled). Predictors of social anxiety. Multiple levels of social functioning contributed to the prediction of social anxiety, accounting for 27% of the variance (Table 3). Although gender was not related to social anxiety initially, once other variables were entered, girls were observed to report more social anxiety than boys (see Step 5). In addition, adolescents who affiliated with a peer crowd—both the high- and the low-status crowds—reported lower levels of social anxiety. Also, higher levels of relational victimization, lower levels of positive best friendship qualities, and higher levels of negative best friendship qualities each uniquely added to the prediction of social anxiety. Finally, adolescents who were not dating reported more social anxiety than those who were dating (Step 4); however, once dating was accounted for, the qualities of the romantic relationship did not predict social anxiety (Step 5). Overall, the general peer relation variables (peer crowd and peer victimization) made the largest contribution to the prediction of adolescents’ social anxiety. In particular, relational victimization was a substantial predictor and remained so even when the other variables were subsequently entered. To evaluate whether gender moderated any of these main effects, interaction terms were computed between gender and each predictor variable and tested in the regression model after the main effects (i.e., after Step 5). However, none of the gender interactions were significant. For exploratory purposes, interactions between the predictors and ethnicity were also tested, using the two largest ethnic groups, Hispanics (n = 282) and nonHispanic Whites (n = 73). The pattern of main effects was identical to that for the full sample (as in Table 3). In addition, 3 of 10 interactions with ethnicity were significant. First, although relational victimization was related to social anxiety for both ethnic groups, this relationship was stronger for White (β = .63) than for Latino adolescents (β = .34; interaction β = –.26, p < .05). Second, less positive interactions with romantic part- Table 3. Hierarchical Regression Predicting Social Anxiety From Peer and Relationship Variables Variables Step 1 Gender (girls = 1) Step 2 General peer relations Popular/jock Burnout/alternative Relational victimization Overt victimization Step 3 Best friendships Positive—best friend Negative—best friend Step 4 Dating Dating status (1= dating) Step 5 Romantic relationships Gender Popular/jock Burnout/alternative Relational victimization Overt victimization Positive—best friend Negative—best friend Dating status Positive—romantic partner Negative—romantic partner R2 ∆ .00 B SE B β .016 1.18 .00 .22 –4.35 –4.06 1.64 –.26 1.17 1.53 .18 .27 –.17*** –.12** .45*** –.05 –2.34 2.58 .78 .98 –.14** .12** –2.72 1.05 –.11* 2.52 –3.45 –4.29 1.54 –.30 –2.21 2.09 –.87 –.84 .71 1.14 1.16 1.49 .18 .27 .78 1.04 3.04 .83 1.21 .11* –.14** –.13** .43*** –.06 –.13** .10* –.04 –.12 .04 .04 .01 .00 F Step F Model .00 .00 29.19*** 23.36*** 10.05*** 20.28*** 6.66** 18.82*** .69 15.17*** *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 55 LA GRECA AND HARRISON Table 4. Hierarchical Regression Predicting Depressive Symptoms From Peer and Relationship Variables Variables Step 1 Gender (girls = 1) Step 2 General peer relations Popular/jock Burnout/alternative Relational victimization Overt victimization Step 3 Best friendships Positive—best friend Negative—best friend Step 4 Dating status Dating (dating = 1) Step 5 Romantic relationships Gender Popular/jock Burnout/alternative Relational victimization Overt victimization Positive—best friend Negative—best friend Dating Positive—romantic partner Negative—romantic partner R2 ∆ B SE B β .01 1.55 .99 .08 –2.55 .27 .86 .55 1.02 1.34 .16 .23 –.12* .01 .28*** .13* –.05 3.27 .69 .86 .00 .19*** .09 .94 .01 3.07 –2.59 .16 .79 .30 –.04 2.14 –1.42 –.69 3.52 1.00 1.02 1.31 .16 .24 .68 .92 2.67 .73 1.06 .15 .03 .00 .02 F Step F Model 2.46 2.46 18.08*** 15.04*** 7.56** 13.24*** .01 11.56*** 6.05** 10.68*** .15** –.12* .00 .26*** .07 .00 .12* –.07 –.12 .24** *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. ners was related to higher social anxiety only for Latino adolescents (β = – .25; interaction β = –.30, p < .02). Finally, dating was associated with lower social anxiety primarily for Latino adolescents (β = –.19; interaction β = –.33, p < .005). Predictors of depressive symptoms. Similar analyses were conducted for depressive symptoms (Table 4). All aspects of social functioning contributed to adolescents’ depressive symptoms, accounting for 21% of the total variance. Although gender was not related to depressive symptoms initially, once other variables were entered, girls were found to report more depressive symptoms than boys (see Step 5). Adolescents who affiliated with a high status peer crowd reported fewer depressive symptoms, whereas higher levels of relational victimization predicted more depressive symptoms. In addition, higher levels of negative friendship qualities added to the prediction of depressive symptoms (see Step 3). Of interest, overt victimization initially predicted depressive symptoms (see Step 2), but was no longer significant when negative friendship qualities were entered (Step 3). Finally, in contrast to findings for social anxiety, dating did not predict depressive symptoms (Step 4), but romantic relationship qualities did (Step 5). Adolescents who reported more negative interactions in romantic relationships reported higher levels of depression. Overall, the general peer relation variables (peer crowd and peer 56 victimization) made the largest contribution to the prediction of depressive symptoms, and, in particular, relational victimization was a substantial predictor even when the other variables were entered. Gender was evaluated as a moderator of the main effects, by testing additional interaction terms (after Step 5). However, none of the gender interactions were significant. Again, for exploratory purposes, interactions between the predictors and ethnicity were tested for the Hispanic and White adolescents. Two significant interactions emerged. Overt victimization was related to depressive symptoms for White (β = .33) but not for Latino adolescents (β = .01; interaction β = –.43, p < .009). Also, although negative interactions with romantic partners were related to depression for all adolescents, this relation was stronger for White (β = .51) than for Latino adolescents (β = .22; interaction β= –36, p < .02). Additional analyses of moderation effects. Because the main findings revealed strong effects for peer victimization, we examined whether the positive qualities of best friendships or romantic relationships moderated the negative effects of peer victimization (both relational and overt). Four additional interaction terms were tested in the regression analyses for social anxiety and depression (i.e., after Step 5). However, none of the interactions terms reached significance. PEER RELATIONS Discussion It has long been recognized that adolescents’ peer relations and friendships contribute in important ways to their psychosocial functioning. However, considerably less is known about the unique ways that romantic relationships contribute to adolescents’ adjustment, above and beyond the contributions of other close friendships. This study addressed some of the gaps in our understanding of the connections between multiple aspects of adolescents’ social worlds and their feelings of social anxiety and depression. To our knowledge, this is the first study to concurrently examine multiple aspects of adolescents’ peer relations (peer crowds, peer victimization, best friendships, romantic relationships) as potential predictors of social anxiety and depressive symptoms. Some of the key findings that emerged are that multiple aspects of adolescents’ peer relations are important and unique predictors of internal distress, that peer victimization (i.e., relational victimization) is a salient factor contributing to adolescents’ social anxiety and depressive affect, and that there are similarities and differences in the pattern of associations between adolescents’ social functioning and their reports of social anxiety and depression. These key issues are discussed in the sections that follow. General Peer Relations: Peer Crowd Affiliations Adolescents’ peer crowd affiliation, and in particular belonging to a high-status crowd, may confer some protection against feelings of social anxiety and depression. This is not surprising, in that adolescents who affiliate with high-status crowds may be regarded highly by their peers (La Greca et al., 2001; Prinstein & La Greca, 1998) and may be connected to a socially oriented network that facilitates social interactions (Brown et al., 1986). It is noteworthy that the “benefit” of affiliating with a high-status crowd appears to be independent of having good-quality friendships and romantic relationships. Interestingly, belonging to a low-status peer crowd was also a protective factor for social anxiety, but not for depression. Brown et al. (1986) found that even adolescents from low-status peer crowds reported that support, friendship, and companionship opportunities were some of the positive benefits of crowd affiliation. Thus, it is possible that some of these positive qualities (e.g., opportunities for companionship) protect adolescents from feeling socially anxious, even if their peer crowd is not highly regarded. In fact, some evidence suggests that the Alternative crowd may be especially close-knit, with 94% of such teens having one or more close friends who also affiliate with the same crowd (La Greca et al., 2001). Alternatively, socially anxious teens may be unlikely to affiliate with the Burnout or Alternative crowds, because such teens engage in “acting out” and problem behaviors (e.g., substance use, fighting; La Greca et al., 2001; Prinstein et al., 2001). In contrast, affiliation with low-status peer crowds did not protect adolescents against feelings of depression; such affiliation was unrelated to depressive symptoms. Future research that examines the differential effects of low-status peer crowd affiliation will be important. General Peer Relations: Peer Victimization In terms of social risk factors for internal distress, the most consistent finding of the study concerns peer victimization. In particular, relational victimization was substantially and significantly related to adolescents’ reports of social anxiety and depression, even when negative aspects of adolescents’ close friendships and romantic relationships were considered. Also troubling were the results of analyses indicating that positive qualities of best friendships and romantic relationships did not buffer the negative impact of peer victimization. These findings add to the accumulating evidence regarding the aversive and destructive nature of relational and reputation-based forms of peer victimization. In concert with other findings, the data suggest that peer victimization may lead to feelings of internal distress. For example, Vernberg et al. (1992) demonstrated that aversive peer experiences prospectively predicted increases in early adolescents’ social anxiety, although social anxiety did not predict increases in negative peer experiences. Peer rejection and negative peer experiences have long been thought to play a causal role in the development of social anxiety among adolescents (La Greca, 2001; La Greca & Lopez, 1998). This study also demonstrated a linkage between relational victimization and depressive affect, extending recent work by Prinstein et al. (2001). Other prior work found reciprocal influences between aversive peer experiences and early adolescents’ feelings of depression (Vernberg, 1990). Together these findings suggest that aversive, exclusionary, relational types of peer victimization (e.g., excluding a teen from activities) play an important role in the development of adolescents’ feelings of depression, although it is also possible that teens exclude and victimize adolescents who are psychologically distressed. Studies of children have recently shown reciprocal influences between peer victimization and internal distress (e.g., Hodges & Perry, 1999). Longitudinal studies of adolescents also will be critical to understanding the nature of the causal pathways between relational victimization and depressive affect. 57 LA GRECA AND HARRISON As in other studies of adolescents (Prinstein et al., 2001), both boys and girls reported more relational than overt victimization. Furthermore, boys reported significantly more relational victimization than girls. In contrast, some studies of preadolescents show higher levels of relational victimization among girls (e.g., Crick & Bigbee, 1998). Our findings may suggest a developmental shift in terms of boys’ aggression against peers. By mid-adolescence, boys are using more sophisticated and less overt peer victimization strategies. From a clinical perspective, these findings are troubling, as they suggest that victimization from the general peer group—and not just negative interactions with close friends and romantic partners—contribute in significant ways to adolescents’ feelings of social anxiety and depression. Yet, relational forms of victimization are very hard for teachers, parents, and other concerned adults to detect and ameliorate. Many adolescents do not disclose such events to others, and, when they do, peers and siblings are more likely to be chosen as confidants than adults (Vernberg, Ewell, Beery, Freeman, & Abwender, 1995). Moreover, it is not clear whether school-based programs to reduce adolescent problem behavior, including bullying and peer victimization, address the surreptitious and hurtful nature of relational victimization (e.g., Perry et al., 2003). In this study, overt victimization was less consistently related to adolescents’ internal distress. Specifically, overt victimization was related to social anxiety and depression (in correlation analyses), but when it was considered simultaneously with relational victimization its unique contribution was less clear. Moreover, it appeared that overt victimization predicted depressive symptoms for White adolescents but not for Hispanic adolescents. Given that the White adolescents in our sample represented a “statistical minority” (i.e., they were in the minority within their school and the county), it is possible that peer victimization has a greater impact on adolescents who are in the minority, regardless of their cultural or ethnic background. In general, the pattern of findings suggests that overt victimization may contribute to depressive symptoms for some adolescents, and further research examining this issue will be important and desirable. Overall, the findings further suggest that both relational and overt victimization are part of a constellation of negative social experiences; they are interrelated and also significantly associated with negative interactions in best friendships and romantic relationships. Thus, adolescents who are victimized by peers are likely to have negative, aversive experiences with their peers, their friends, and also their romantic partners. These findings underscore the need for preventive interventions that address relational forms of victimization and that also consider the multiple levels of vic58 timization (peers, friends, romantic partners) that some teens encounter. Moreover, research that examines relational and overt victimization in the context of romantic relationships will be extremely important, as many adolescents report having a violent episode with a dating partner (Bergman, 1992; Smith & William, 1992). Best Friendships Several important findings emerged regarding the role of best friendships in understanding adolescents’ internal distress. Specifically, adolescents with more positive qualities in their best friendships reported less social anxiety, even when other aspects of their peer relations were considered. This suggests that a goodquality best friendship may serve a protective function, at least in terms of adolescents’ feelings of social anxiety. Interestingly, positive friendship qualities did not serve a protective function for depressive symptoms. Future research should examine this issue further. It is possible that the qualities of best friendships (support, disclosure) may actually reinforce depressive feelings for some adolescents, if the adolescents discuss and revisit problems and focus on negative feelings, a process that has been referred to as co-rumination (Rose, 2002). In contrast, the negative qualities of best friendships predicted feelings of depression and social anxiety. Adolescents who reported high levels of negative qualities in their best friendships (e.g., exclusion, pressure) were more depressed and socially anxious. It was not possible to determine whether negative interactions contributed to feelings of depression and social anxiety, or whether adolescents who were depressed or anxious behaved negatively with friends or perceived friends’ behavior negatively. Nevertheless, the findings point to interpersonal difficulties in the close friendships of adolescents who feel depressed or socially anxious. Clinically, the findings support the emphasis on improving the interpersonal skills and relationships of socially anxious adolescents (e.g., Beidel & Turner, 1998) and depressed adolescents (e.g., Mufson et al., 1999). Furthermore, efforts to improve adolescents’ interpersonal functioning may be critical for preventing clinically significant social anxiety and depressive affect. Romantic Relationships A final aspect of social relations addressed in this study—the presence and qualities of romantic relationships—revealed several key findings. First, adolescents who were not involved in a romantic relationship, particularly Hispanic youth, were more socially anxious than those who were dating or romantically involved. Dating is a normative experience for adoles- PEER RELATIONS cents, and by 16 years of age, most adolescents report having had a romantic relationship (Carver et al., 2003; Feiring, 1996). At the same time, the development of romantic relationships is a new and potentially stressful social task, and many adolescents report distress regarding how they should behave (Neider & SeiffgeKrenke, 2001). It is possible that adolescents who are not dating feel anxious around peers because dating contributes to their status and belonging in their peer crowd (Connolly et al., 1999; Levesque, 1993). It is also possible that socially anxious adolescents may feel uncomfortable in dating situations and delay or avoid dating (Glickman & La Greca, 2004). However, dating status was unrelated to depressive symptoms. Second, in terms of the qualities of romantic relationships, negative qualities predicted depressive symptoms (but not social anxiety), even when other peer relations and best friend qualities were considered. This pattern is important, as it indicates that the negative qualities of romantic relationships make a unique contribution to understanding adolescents’ depressive affect, even though qualities of best friendships and romantic relationships are interrelated (e.g., Kuttler & La Greca, 2004). Moreover, the findings are consistent with other emerging work on adolescent romantic relationships (Davila et al., 2004) that found an association between romantic involvement and depressive symptoms and suggested that “romantic stress” may mediate this relationship. Our findings support the notion that the “stress” of a romantic relationship contributes to depressive affect, as the presence of a dating relationship did not relate to depressive symptoms but negative interactions in the relationship did. A few findings for ethnicity are worth highlighting. Although negative relationship qualities were related to depression for all adolescents, this pattern was stronger for White than Hispanic adolescents. There was also some indication that the absence of a dating relationship contributed to greater social anxiety for Hispanic youth. Although exploratory, the results suggest that future research on adolescent romantic relationships and psychosocial functioning may benefit from a careful examination of ethnicity as an important moderating variable. In addition, longitudinal research will be essential for understanding the direction of effects (i.e., negative interactions lead to depression, or depressed adolescents interact negatively with partners) and to consider potential reciprocal influences. The pattern of findings obtained here does raise the question of whether dating in adolescence, particularly when there are problems in the relationship, represents a psychosocial hazard for adolescents. Conclusions and Future Directions The findings indicate that multiple aspects of adolescents’ peer relations and close relationships contrib- ute to feelings of internal distress in cumulative fashion. Adolescents’ peer crowd affiliations, positive interactions with best friends, and the presence of a dating relationship appear to “protect” adolescents against feelings of social anxiety, whereas relational victimization and negative interactions in best friendships may contribute to feelings of social anxiety. In contrast, adolescents’ affiliation with a high-status peer crowd appeared to afford some protection against depressive affect, but relational victimization and negative qualities of best friendships and romantic relationships were key factors associated with depressive symptoms. In particular, relational victimization was a substantial predictor of both social anxiety and depressive symptoms. A few findings varied by ethnicity, but the overall patterns were similar across the main ethnic groups represented in the sample. Some of the ethnicity-related findings pertained to the potential effects of dating and romantic relationships, suggesting that further study of ethnic and cultural differences in romantic relationships may be important in the future. In addition, investigation of the potential differential effects of peer victimization for adolescents of varying ethnic or minority group status would be of interest. Interestingly, the patterns of association between adolescents’ social relations and internal distress did not differ by gender. Girls reported more social anxiety and depression than boys when other variables were controlled, and gender differences in dating status (i.e., girls were more likely to be dating) and relationship qualities (i.e., girls reported more positive interactions) were consistent with earlier research (e.g., Carver et al., 2003). Thus, it is possible that factors other than social relations contribute to the differential rates of social anxiety and depression that have been reported for adolescent girls and boys. In general, the findings suggest that general peer relations (peer crowd affiliations and peer victimization) and the qualities of best friendships and romantic relationships may play a similar role in boys’and girls’reports of social anxiety and depression. Despite important contributions, the results should be interpreted with the following cautions in mind. First, the findings were obtained from a community sample of adolescents. Thus, the extent to which the findings generalize to clinical levels of social anxiety or depression is unknown. Second, the sample included a diverse ethnic mix of adolescents, with a large representation of Hispanic youth. Individuals of Hispanic ethnicity are the largest and fastest-growing ethnic group in the United States and thus are important to study. In general, the overall results were similar for Hispanic and White youth, and the findings were consistent with studies that have focused on predominantly White (e.g., Vernberg et al., 1992) and ethnically diverse adolescents (e.g., Prinstein et al., 2001). Nevertheless, it will be useful to 59 LA GRECA AND HARRISON evaluate potential ethnic and cultural differences in greater detail in future studies, particularly with respect to the roles of peer victimization and of dating and romantic relationships in adolescents’ adjustment. Third, information was obtained solely from the adolescents. Although adolescents are considered to be the best informants for friendships and social relationships, it would be informative to incorporate peer or parent reports in future work. Finally, the study provided a one-time snapshot of adolescents’ peer crowds, peer victimization, best friendships, and dating relationships. Longitudinal designs will be essential for capturing the dynamic nature of social relationships and for examining causal processes associated with feelings of social anxiety and depression. References Aalto-Setaelae, T., Marttunen, M., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Poikolainen, K., & Loennqvist, J. (2002). Depressive symptoms in adolescence as predictors of early adulthood depressive disorders and maladjustment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1235–1237. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Manual for the Revised Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100. Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (1998). Shy children, phobic adults: Nature and treatment of social phobia. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Berndt, T. J. (1982). The features and effects of friendship in early adolescence. Child Development, 53, 1447–1460. Bergman, L. (1992). Dating violence among high school students. Social Work, 37, 21–27. Berndt, T. J., & Keefe, K. (1995). Friends’ influence on adolescents’ adjustment to school. Child Development, 66, 1312–1329. Birmaher, B., Ryan, N. D., Douglas, E. W., Brent, D. A., Kaufman, J., Dahl, R. E., Perel, J., & Nelson, B. (1996). Childhood and adolescent depression: A review of the past 10 years. Part I. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1427–1439. Brown, B. B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 171–196). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. Brown, B. B., Eicher, S. A., & Petrie, S. (1986). The importance of peer group (“crowd”) affiliation in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 9, 73–96. Brown, B. B., & Lohr, M. J. (1987). Peer-group affiliation and adolescent self-esteem: An integration of ego-identity and symbolic-interaction theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 47–55. Buhrmester, D. (1990). Intimacy of friendship, interpersonal competence, and adjustment during preadolescence and adolescence. Child Development, 61, 1101–1111. Carver, K., Joyner, K., & Udry, J. R. (2003). National estimates of adolescent romantic relationships. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adolescent romantic relationships and sexual behavior: Theory, research, and practical implications (pp. 291–329). New York: Cambridge University. 60 Collins, W. A. (2003). More than myth: The developmental significance of romantic relationships during adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 1–24. Compas, B. E., Slavin, L. A., Wagner, B. M., & Vannatta, K. (1986). Relationship of life events and social support with psychological dysfunction among adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 15, 205–221. Connolly, J. A., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, D. (1999). Conceptions of cross-sex friendships and romantic relationships in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 481–494. Connolly, J. A., Furman, W., & Konarski, R. (2000). The role of peers in the emergence of heterosexual romantic relationships in adolescence. Child Development, 71, 1395–1408. Crick, N. R., & Bigbee, M. A. (1998). Relational and overt forms of peer victimization: A multi-informant approach. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 337–347. Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1996). Children’s treatment by peers: Victims of relational and overt aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 367–380. Daley, S. E., & Hammen, C. (2002). Depressive symptoms and close relationships during the transition to adulthood: Perspectives from dysphoric women, their best friends, and their romantic partners. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 129–141. Davila, J., Steinberg, S. J., Kachadourian, L., Cobb, R., & Fincham, F. (2004). Romantic involvement and depressive symptoms in early and late adolescence: The role of preoccupied relational style. Personal Relationships, 11, 161–178. Devine, D., Kempton, T., & Forehand, R. (1994). Adolescent depressed mood and young adult functioning: A longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, 629–640. Dolcini, M. M., & Adler, N. E. (1994). Perceived competencies, peer group affiliation, and risk behavior among early adolescents. Health Psychology, 13, 496–506. Feiring, C. (1996). Concepts of romance in fifteen-year-old adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 181–200. Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1016–1024. Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in perceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child Development, 63, 103–115. Glickman, A. R., & La Greca, A. M. (2004). The Dating Anxiety Scale for Adolescents: Scale development and associations with adolescent functioning. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 566–578. Harter, S. (1997). The development of self-representations. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., Vol. III). New York: Wiley. Hecht, D. B., Inderbitzen, H. M., & Bukowski, A. L. (1998). The relationship between peer status and depressive symptoms in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 153–60. Hodges, E. V., & Perry, D. G. (1999). Personal and interpersonal antecedents and consequences of victimization by peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 677–685. Inderbitzen, H. M., Walters, K. S., & Bukowski, A. L. (1997). The role of social anxiety in adolescent peer relations: Differences among sociometric status groups and rejected subgroups. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 338–348. Inderbitzen-Nolan, H. M., & Walters, K. S. (2000). Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents: Normative data and further evidence of construct validity. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 360–371. Kuttler, A. F., & La Greca, A. M. (2004). Adolescents’ romantic relationships: Do they help or hinder close friendships? Journal of Adolescence, 27, 395–414. PEER RELATIONS La Greca, A. M. (1999). Manual for the Social Anxiety Scales. Miami, FL: Author. La Greca, A. M. (2001). Friends or foes? Peer influences on anxiety among children and adolescents. In W. K. Silverman & P. D. A. Treffers (Eds.), Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents (pp. 159–186.) Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. La Greca, A. M., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents: Linkages with peer relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 83–94. La Greca, A. M., & Prinstein, M. J. (1999). The peer group. In W. K. Silverman & T. H. Ollendick (Eds.), Developmental issues in the clinical treatment of children and adolescents (pp. 171–198.) Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. La Greca, A. M., Prinstein, M. J., & Fetter, M. (2001). Adolescent peer crowd affiliation: Linkages with health-risk behaviors and close friendships. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 26, 131–143. Laursen, B. (1996). Closeness and conflict in adolescent peer relationships: Interdependence with friends and romantic partners. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. H. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 186–212). New York: Cambridge University Press. Levesque, R. J. R. (1993). The romantic experiences of adolescents in satisfying love relationships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 219–251. Lewinsohn, P. M., Roberts, R. E., Seeley, J. R., Rohde, P., Gotlib, I. H., & Hops, H. (1994). Adolescent psychopathology: II. Psychosocial risk factors for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 302–315. Mosbach, P., & Leventhal, H. L. (1988). Peer group identification and smoking: Implications for intervention. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 238–245. Moutier, C. Y., & Stein, M. B. (1999). The history, epidemiology, and diagnosis of social anxiety disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60, 4–8. Mufson, L., Weissman, M. M., Moreau, D., & Garfinkel, R. (1999). Efficacy of interpersonal psychotherapy for depressed adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 573–579. Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simons-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among US youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 2094–2100. Neider, T., & Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2001). Coping with stress in different phases of romantic development. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 297–311. Perry, C. L., Komro, K. A., Veblen-Mortenson, S., Bosma, L. M., Farbakhsh, K., Munson, K. A., et al. (2003). A randomized controlled trial of the middle and junior high school D.A.R.E. and D.A.R.E. Plus programs. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 157, 178–184. Petersen, A. C., Compas, B. E., Brooks-Gunn, J., Stemmler, M., Ey, S., & Grant, K. E. (1993). Depression in adolescence. American Psychologist, 48, 155–168. Phillips, L., Hughes, P., & Wilkes, K. (1998, February). Black students’peer crowds: An exploratory study. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Adolescence, San Diego, CA. Prinstein, M. J., Boergers, J., & Vernberg, E. M. (2001). Overt and relational aggression in adolescents: Social-psychological adjustment of aggressors and victims. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, 479–491. Prinstein, M. J., & La Greca, A. M. (1998, February). Adolescents’ peer groups and close friendships: Are they one and the same? Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research on Adolescence, San Diego, CA. Prinstein, M. J., & La Greca, A. M. (2002). Peer crowd affiliation and internalizing distress in adolescence: A longitudinal follow-back study. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12, 325–351. Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys. Child Development, 73, 1830–1843. Smith, J., & William, J. (1992). From abusive household to dating violence. Journal of Family Violence, 7, 153–165. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton. Sussman, S., Dent, C., Stacy, A., Burciaga, C., Raynor, A., Turner, G., et al. (1990). Peer group association and adolescent tobacco use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 349–352. Urberg, K. A., Degirmencioglu, S. M., Tolson, J. M., & HallidayScher, K. (1995). The structure of adolescent peer networks. Developmental Psychology, 31, 540–547. Vernberg, E. M. (1990). Psychological adjustment and experiences with peers during early adolescence: Reciprocal, incidental, or unidirectional relationships? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 187–198. Vernberg, E. M., Abwender, D. A., Ewell, K. K., & Beery, S. H. (1992). Social anxiety and peer relationships in early adolescence: A prospective analysis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 189–196. Vernberg, E. M., Ewell, K. K., Beery, S. H., Freeman, C. M., & Abwender, D. A. (1995). Aversive exchanges with peers and adjustment during early adolescence: Is disclosure helpful? Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 26, 43–56. Vernberg, E. M., Jacobs, A. K., & Hershberger, S. L. (1999). Peer victimization and attitudes about violence during early adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 386–395. Received November 10, 2003 Accepted July 13, 2004 61

    Calculate your order
    Pages (275 words)
    Standard price: $0.00
    Client Reviews
    4.9
    Sitejabber
    4.6
    Trustpilot
    4.8
    Our Guarantees
    100% Confidentiality
    Information about customers is confidential and never disclosed to third parties.
    Original Writing
    We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
    Timely Delivery
    No missed deadlines – 97% of assignments are completed in time.
    Money Back
    If you're confident that a writer didn't follow your order details, ask for a refund.

    Calculate the price of your order

    You will get a personal manager and a discount.
    We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
    Total price:
    $0.00
    Power up Your Academic Success with the
    Team of Professionals. We’ve Got Your Back.
    Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.
    WeCreativez WhatsApp Support
    Our customer support team is here to answer your questions. Ask us anything!
    👋 Hi, how can I help?